January 2015
Volume 4, Issue 1
Free
Letters to the Editor  |   January 2015
Extraordinary Claims Require Extraordinary Evidence: Centrally Mediated Preservation of Binocular Visual Field in Glaucoma is Unlikely
Author Affiliations & Notes
  • Jonathan Denniss
    Visual Neuroscience Group, School of Psychology, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, United Kingdom
  • Paul H. Artes
    Ophthalmology & Visual Sciences, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada
  • Correspondence: Jonathan Denniss, Visual Neuroscience Group, School of Psychology, University of Notthingham, University Park, Notthingham NG7 2RD, UK; jonathan.denniss@nottingham.ac.uk 
Translational Vision Science & Technology January 2015, Vol.4, 3. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/tvst.4.1.3
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Jonathan Denniss, Paul H. Artes; Extraordinary Claims Require Extraordinary Evidence: Centrally Mediated Preservation of Binocular Visual Field in Glaucoma is Unlikely. Trans. Vis. Sci. Tech. 2015;4(1):3. https://doi.org/10.1167/tvst.4.1.3.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
We have read with interest the recent article by Sponsel et al.1 There is much evidence that glaucomatous damage occurs at the optic nerve head,2 and therefore we were surprised by the authors' conjecture that there may be a central mechanism that preserves the binocular visual field in advanced glaucoma. 
If indeed there were some central mechanism responsible for “interlocking” monocular visual field defects to preserve binocular function, patients' binocular damage should, on average, be less severe than would be expected if the spatial pattern of damage in both fellow eyes were independent. We tested this simple hypothesis as follows: visual field pairs (24-2, Humphrey Field Analyzer; Carl Zeiss Meditec, Dublin, CA) from patients with glaucoma (mean deviation worse than −2.5 dB in both eyes) were taken from the datasets of previous studies. One dataset (n = 2463) was from Halifax, Canada; the second (n = 854,5), from Rotterdam, the Netherlands, is freely available from http://www.orgids.com/ (last accessed 7/31/2014). From both datasets, we selected the most recent pair of right and left visual fields from each patient. Similar to Sponsel et al.,1 we calculated the integrated visual field6 as a proxy measure of the true binocular visual field for each of these patients (total n = 331). The greater of the two monocular sensitivities was used to represent the “binocular” sensitivity at each location, and the mean sensitivity (MSTRUE, in dB) was derived as a summary index. Then, for comparison, we derived the distributions of the binocular sensitivities (MSRANDOM, dB) that would have been obtained if the right visual field had been paired with the left visual field of 20 different patients who had left MS within ± 2.0 dB of the true left MS This was possible for n = 298 patients. 
By comparing MSTRUE with MSRANDOM, a distribution of differences (MSTRUE − MSRANDOM) is obtained for each individual patient. Under the null hypothesis of randomness, MSTRUE will be similar to MSRANDOM (mean difference ≈ 0). If indeed there existed a central mechanism that minimizes binocular damage, the MSTRUE should be systematically better than MSRANDOM (mean difference > 0). 
We found no evidence for such an effect (Fig. 1). In fact, true binocular visual fields were typically slightly worse than the integrated fields derived from randomly matched pairs (median difference between MSTRUE and MSRANDOM, −0.4 dB, Wilcoxon P < 0.001). This effect appeared to increase with visual field damage, and it is probably explained by the common disease process and predisposition of anatomically similar fellow eyes. Coexisting neurological damage (e.g., from strokes) would also cause homonymous visual field damage and contribute to this effect. 
Figure 1. 
 
MSTRUE versus mean difference in MS (MSTRUE – MSRANDOM) for 298 patients with glaucoma. Error bars representing ±1 SEM are shown for 50 randomly selected patients distributed across the range of MSTRUE. The data are not suggestive of binocular visual field preservation. A small tendency is shown for MSTRUE to be worse than MSRANDOM (median difference in MS −0.4 dB, interquartile range −0.8 to 0.0 dB).
Figure 1. 
 
MSTRUE versus mean difference in MS (MSTRUE – MSRANDOM) for 298 patients with glaucoma. Error bars representing ±1 SEM are shown for 50 randomly selected patients distributed across the range of MSTRUE. The data are not suggestive of binocular visual field preservation. A small tendency is shown for MSTRUE to be worse than MSRANDOM (median difference in MS −0.4 dB, interquartile range −0.8 to 0.0 dB).
A power analysis (Fig. 2) suggests that, with our approach, samples of n = 100 would provide ample power to detect even small amounts (∼1 dB) of binocular visual field preservation if such an effect had existed. Thus, the small effect in the opposite direction suggests that centrally mediated binocular visual field preservation is unlikely in glaucoma. 
Figure 2. 
 
Power to detect differences between MSTRUE and MSRANDOM of varying magnitude when n = 100 and between-patient SD of differences is as shown, corresponding approximately to MSTRUE of 15, 20, and 25 dB. A study of this size is powered to detect even a small amount of binocular visual field preservation, should it exist.
Figure 2. 
 
Power to detect differences between MSTRUE and MSRANDOM of varying magnitude when n = 100 and between-patient SD of differences is as shown, corresponding approximately to MSTRUE of 15, 20, and 25 dB. A study of this size is powered to detect even a small amount of binocular visual field preservation, should it exist.
Extraordinary claims require extraordinary evidence. In a paper published nearly 10 years ago, Ioannidis7 explained “why most published research findings are false”: hypotheses with a low prior probability of being true require strong evidence to generate a post-study probability of being true greater than that of being false, but many researchers are mislead by hypothesis tests (P values) into overestimating the strength of their evidence.7 While we disagree on binocular visual field preservation in glaucoma, we thank Sponsel et al.1 for a stimulating paper. The question of how, and when, visual field damage impairs real-world visual performance is one of the most important topics in glaucoma, and we hope that many other groups will contribute to this discussion. 
References
Sponsel WE Groth SL Satsangi N. Maddess T. Reilly MA Refined data analysis provides clinical evidence for central nervous system control of chronic glaucomatous neurodegeneration. Trans Vis Sci Tech. 2014: 3: 1. [CrossRef]
Quigley HA Glaucoma. Lancet. 2011: 377: 1367– 1377. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Chauhan BC Malik R. Shuba LM Rafuse PE Nicolela MT Artes PH Rates of glaucomatous visual field change in a large clinical population. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2014: 55: 4135– 4143. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bryan SR Vermeer KA Eilers P.C. Lemij HG Lesaffre EMEH Robust and censored modeling and prediction of progression in glaucomatous visual fields. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2013: 54: 6694– 6670. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Erler NS Bryan SR Eilers P.C. Lesaffre EMEH Lemij HG Vermeer KA Optimizing structure-function relationship by maximizing correspondence between glaucomatous visual fields and mathematical retinal nerve fiber models. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2014: 55: 2350– 2357. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Crabb DP Viswanathan A. McNaught AI Poinoosawmy D. Fitzke FW Hitchings RA Simulating binocular visual field status in glaucoma. Br J Ophthalmol. 1998: 82: 1236– 1241. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ioannidis J.A. Why most published research findings are false. PLoS Med. 2005: 2: e124. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. 
 
MSTRUE versus mean difference in MS (MSTRUE – MSRANDOM) for 298 patients with glaucoma. Error bars representing ±1 SEM are shown for 50 randomly selected patients distributed across the range of MSTRUE. The data are not suggestive of binocular visual field preservation. A small tendency is shown for MSTRUE to be worse than MSRANDOM (median difference in MS −0.4 dB, interquartile range −0.8 to 0.0 dB).
Figure 1. 
 
MSTRUE versus mean difference in MS (MSTRUE – MSRANDOM) for 298 patients with glaucoma. Error bars representing ±1 SEM are shown for 50 randomly selected patients distributed across the range of MSTRUE. The data are not suggestive of binocular visual field preservation. A small tendency is shown for MSTRUE to be worse than MSRANDOM (median difference in MS −0.4 dB, interquartile range −0.8 to 0.0 dB).
Figure 2. 
 
Power to detect differences between MSTRUE and MSRANDOM of varying magnitude when n = 100 and between-patient SD of differences is as shown, corresponding approximately to MSTRUE of 15, 20, and 25 dB. A study of this size is powered to detect even a small amount of binocular visual field preservation, should it exist.
Figure 2. 
 
Power to detect differences between MSTRUE and MSRANDOM of varying magnitude when n = 100 and between-patient SD of differences is as shown, corresponding approximately to MSTRUE of 15, 20, and 25 dB. A study of this size is powered to detect even a small amount of binocular visual field preservation, should it exist.
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×