Our study provides the estimated limits of agreement between cone densities derived from gaze-directed single AO frames and those obtained from field-determined localization on wide-field AO montages. Although there was no statistically significant difference in cone densities derived from the two methods, the limits of agreement were relatively wide. These findings have significant implications on the comparability of cone density values from clinical trials that use different methodologies to sample the area of interest for cone density measurement.
The proportions of sampling windows that were discarded before analysis due to poor image quality were 43% when taken from single AO image frames, and 40% when taken from the wide-field AO montages. Debellemanière et al.
11 reported exclusion of at least one eye from 46.9% of subjects due to inadequate AO image quality in their study of patients taking hydroxychloroquine without any evidence of maculopathy. Further, in a separate study of AO images obtained from 72 eyes of healthy subjects aged 14 to 69 years, the images from 52.7% of subjects were excluded from analysis due to image quality that was insufficient to allow automatic montage and/or cone counting, including two patients who were aged under 30 with no apparent ocular pathology.
12 Both studies used the same commercial instrument for AO image acquisition as the one used in our study. It is unclear what causes these relatively high rates of sampling window failing image quality control, given that factors known to cause difficulty with AO imaging, such as macular pathology, media opacity, and extremes of refractive errors, have been excluded from the study population. It is possible that a number of higher order aberrations are not corrected adequately or perhaps not corrected at all using this instrument in a significant proportion of people. The variability in obtaining adequate quality AO images in healthy subjects is not limited to flood-illumination optical devices. Using AO-SLO, Li et al.
13 were able to resolve the entire foveal cone mosaic in just four of 18 healthy eyes with varying axial lengths, mostly due to failure to obtain adequate images within 0.03 mm eccentricity of the center of the fovea. Improvements to hardware-based adaptive optics may improve our ability to correct for the eye's wave aberrations, but there also are computational wavefront correction algorithms that have enabled visualization of even highly packed individual cone photoreceptors without the need for expensive, bulky hardware.
14–16 In the foreseeable future, computational correction of images may augment or even replace hardware-based adaptive optics technologies in visualizing human retinal photoreceptor cells.
We demonstrated a misalignment of up to 2° between retinal loci defined by gaze direction and the corresponding field projected retinal loci in three eyes from three randomly selected healthy subjects. In the first and third cases shown, this translation is temporal, whereas in the second case, the translation is nasal and the magnitude of the translation is variable between the subjects and loci. Several studies have described the relationship between fixationally- and anatomically-determined retinal loci using structural modalities other than OCT to identify the fovea centralis. These include AO cone photoreceptor imaging to identify the position at which the greatest peak of cone density occurs
17–19 and fluorescein angiography to identify the central point of the foveal avascular zone.
20 They show that the location of the anatomically-determined fovea center often does not correlate precisely with the centroid of the preferred retinal locus and that the magnitude of the misalignment in these retinal locations varies between subjects. It also has been shown that the center of fixation is displaced from the location of peak foveal cone density as measured by AO imaging by an average of 18 to 34 μm.
17–19 Micro-saccades also contribute to the mismatch of retinal locations between the gaze-directed and anatomically-determined loci of interest. In healthy subjects, it has been estimated that the standard deviation of intertrial fixation position is approximately 17 μm in each of the horizontal and vertical directions.
17,21,22 Theoretical calculations performed previously by Lombardo et al.
23 estimate that the magnitude of the error resulting from displacing the sampling window 18 μm along the horizontal meridian at an eccentricity of approximately 4.3° would be less than 500 cones/mm
2, and that the error is greater towards the foveal center, but still is less than 1000 cones/mm
2 at <1° eccentricity. Therefore, we have designed a prospective study to examine the actual differences in cone density measurements resulting from shift in region of interest due to different sampling methods.
We demonstrated wide limits of agreement between cone densities derived from gaze-directed single AO images and field projection on wide-field AO montage. Our data show a coefficient of variation (SD/mean) of around 10% in comparing the two sampling methods. There are a number of possible explanations for this. The first is misalignment between the retinal loci determined by gaze-direction and field projection on an AO montage. This is due to difference in position of the preferred retinal locus and the anatomic fovea as discussed above. However, our data and that of others
24 would suggest that the error in cone density measurement should not be as large as that shown by the wide limits of agreement because we only analyzed retinal loci between 3° and 7° where the gradient of change in cone density is only approximately 900 cones/mm
2 per degree of eccentricity (approximately 5% change) and most of the misalignment between the two methods was less than 1°.
25 The second explanation is variable cone visualization due to differences in image quality of the cropped sampling window derived from single AO images and the wide-field AO montage. Sampling windows from the latter would have been derived from two or more overlapping single AO images because these were taken at 1° to 2° apart. The optical Stiles Crawford effect has been shown to affect visualization of cones in AO images by way of changing the position at which the camera is focused over the pupil, so that previously dark spots on an AO frame become bright spots on subsequent AO frames when the camera is aligned at different positions over the pupil.
26 Thus, by overlapping two or more single AO images, a greater number of cone photoreceptors may be visualized as the optically silent nonwave guiding cones in one single AO frame becomes wave-guiding cones in another single AO image taken at slightly different gaze angles. This could account for the tendency for images obtained from sampling windows within the wide-field AO montage to show overall higher cone densities than those from single AO images at most of the study loci. An alternative explanation for this trend is that instead of improved visualization of cones, wide-field AO montage formed by potentially inaccurate stitching of overlapping single AO frames can result in a single cone appearing as two very closely spaced adjacent cones, thus resulting in overestimation of cone density. In certain individuals, the cone count from single AO images exceeded that from wide-field AO montage. This may occur if two separate cones are fused into a single cone during the process of imaging stitching. Another reason for reduced cone count in the wide-field AO montage is the introduction of noise into the montage by stitching together a poor quality single AO image with a good quality single AO image resulting in overall reduced cone visualization. This may contribute to poor image quality in sampling windows. It has been noted previously that image quality within an AO montage created by manual stitching of AO-SLO images may vary between different portions of the montage due to technical factors.
27 The open source MosaicJ montaging software that we use in our dataset assigns a weighted contribution from each tile in regions of overlap.
28 The effect of this is demonstrated across six randomly chosen subjects in
Supplementary Figure S2. In cases where the windows from each tile are of similar image quality, the window from the montage is of comparable contrast. In some cases, the windows from the contributing tiles are of varying quality, resulting in degradation of the quality of the window obtained from the montage versus that from the best tile. We did find a trend of a higher percentage of subjects (62% compared to 52%) with adequate sampling window quality at all 12 study loci when single AO frames were used for deriving cone densities but this was not statistically significant.
Previous reports of cone photoreceptor density calculated from AO images have estimated that at 3° eccentricity, the cone density is 16,000 – 21,000 cones/mm
2.
12,29 At a slightly greater eccentricity of 3.2° (at 3° horizontal and 1° vertical displacement), our results of 18,000 to 19,000 cones/mm
2 are in keeping with these previous measurements, with our study similarly demonstrating a tendency for higher cone density measurements in the temporal and nasal quadrants compared to the superior and inferior quadrants. As expected, our estimates of the average cone density decreased with increasing eccentricity from the fovea. Our cone density measurements at 5° eccentricity, 17,800 and 17,500 cones/mm
2 in the temporal and nasal quadrants, respectively, are within the estimates reported by Feng et al.,
12 which reported estimates of between 16,200 and 20,500 cones/mm
3 depending on which sampling method was used at that locus. At the superior and inferior quadrants, their estimates for cone density are between 14,100 and 19,200 cones/mm
3 and again our estimates of 16,500 and 16,200 cones/mm
3 fell within this range. The relative nasotemporal symmetry that we observed is consistent with results of previous AO imaging
25,27 and histological data
30
Our study had a number of limitations. Firstly, the number of subjects analyzed was small and this may contribute to the wide limits of agreement. Nevertheless, the estimates of cone density and the pattern of cone density changes throughout the regions of the macula studied were within the limits expected when comparing to data from previous reports. Secondly, we examined agreement in cone density measurements at 12 retinal loci within the central 14° field of the macula. The limits of agreement may not be applicable to other retinal loci. Thirdly, our data were derived from automatic calculations of cone densities without manual correction. It has been shown that the high variability seen in automated estimates of cone density can be reduced dramatically by manual correction of the automated cone identification.
25,31,32 Additionally, the determination of whether the sampling windows were of adequate image quality was subjective. Methods for defining image quality objectively have been described,
19,33 but it is known that using objective measures of image quality does not necessarily correlate with subjective assessment.
34 Whether the currently proposed methods of assessing image metrics are superior to subjective preference in selecting the most appropriate sampling window for accurately performing cone metrics remains to be investigated.
In conclusion, we illustrated a high frequency of poor quality sampling windows in a cohort of healthy subjects undergoing AO imaging. There was frequent misalignment between the center of gaze-directed single AO image and theoretical corresponding field-determined retinal loci. Although there was no overall bias in cone density between the two methods, wide limits of agreement were found. Future work is needed to determine the optimal method for alignment and merging the overlapping regions from single AO frames to generate the highest quality image and most reliable cone density measurements. Cone density values derived from these two methods are not interchangeable even in healthy subjects.