Subjects performed the obstacle negotiation task under single- and dual-task conditions. There were 12 walking trials per condition (i.e., 36 trials total), with the order of conditions randomized. In each condition, subjects walked along a 4.5-m long and 1.25-m wide path to an “end gate” consisting of two blue vertical poles (height = 25 cm; diameter = 6 cm) after navigating four dark gray vertical poles (height = 165 cm; diameter = 3.5 cm;
Fig. 1). We positioned the poles 60 cm away from each other in the anterior-posterior direction (i.e., the plane of progression) and varied the pole and end gate positions in the medial-lateral direction on a trial-to-trial basis using one of four predetermined arrangements. This meant that subjects experienced each arrangement for a total of three walking trials. We randomized the order of arrangements on a trial-to-trial basis. An opaque wooden board occluded subjects' vision of the walkway before each trial. The visual occlusion and the use of four, randomized arrangements served to reduce the likelihood of subjects using spatial memory to navigate; thus, ensuring they had to use visual information to complete the task. We instructed subjects to walk at a self-selected speed, to navigate the course without stopping, to take the simplest path through the poles without any part of their body going outside the path's borders, and to avoid contact with the poles. An experimenter demonstrated the task to make sure subjects understood the instructions. Subjects started walking immediately once cued.
In one (count) dual-task condition, subjects walked while counting backward by threes, a common secondary task that increases cognitive load
31 and simulates having a detailed conversation with someone. In the other (visual search) dual-task condition, subjects had to identify the location of a shape at the end of each trial after being cued by an experimenter. In this condition, we positioned four tiles (20 × 15 cm) on the ground on both sides of the path containing the obstacles, each with a different black shape on a white background (see
Fig. 1). We randomly varied the sequence of shapes (plus sign, triangle, circle, and square) on a trial-to-trial basis. This dual task condition purposely forced subjects to temporarily direct their gaze away from the path and poles, simulating real-life situations in which we have to both monitor our walking direction and identify landmarks, a task identified by people with eye disease as challenging.
32
We also had subjects count and perform visual search trials while not walking. These represented count and visual search baseline (or single-task) conditions. For the count single task, subjects counted by threes for a total of 10 seconds in each of three trials. We calculated the number of correct responses in this task and during the dual-task situation, then divided these values by their respective trial durations. For the visual search single task, subjects viewed the shapes for 5 seconds before having their vision blocked in each of 12 trials. We divided the proportion of correct responses in the single- and dual-task situations by their respective average trial durations. To determine how dual tasking affected counting and visual search performance, we calculated a dual task cost (DTC) value
33 for each using the following formula: (dual task – single task) / single task. A negative value represents worse performance in the dual-task situation.
A high-speed, head-mounted, mobile eye-tracker (model H6-HS; Applied Science Laboratories, Bedford, MA) recorded gaze position during the task at 120 Hz. A video camera mounted on the eye tracker recorded the subjects' view of the path at 30 Hz. We calibrated the eye tracker using the system's standard 9-point calibration method. Subjects wore their habitual spectacles, if applicable, during testing in the obstacle task. Two Optotrak Certus (Northern Digital, Inc., Waterloo, Canada) cameras, synchronized with the eye tracker, recorded (at 120 Hz) the time-varying positions of infrared-emitting diodes placed on the obstacles and subject's body. We fixed these markers to the subject's head (using a rigid block at the back of the head and mounted on the eye tracker), chest (midway between the sternal notch and xiphoid process), right shoulder (on the acromion), and bilaterally on the heels, mid-feet (dorsal surface at the approximate level of the metatarsal joint), and above the toes on their shoes.