For the normally sighted subjects, we simulated PFL using head-mounted goggles and placed the prisms over the apertures. When pedestrians appeared outside the simulated residual central field (
Display Formula\({\beta _{t = 0}} = 30^{\circ} \)), lower pedestrian detection rates with longer detection RTs were found, as we observed in the patients with PFL (
Fig. 6a). As compared with the PFL patients, we also observed slightly better performance of the normally sighted subjects with simulated PFL in terms of the RTs (
Fig. 6c), which may be due to their better visual acuity and younger age. It may also be affected by the visibility of the PFL. The edges of the simulated PFL apertures are visible and increase the subjects' awareness, while for the patients there is no such apparent visual clue. The detection rates in the normally sighted subjects with simulated PFL ranged from 47% to 97% (
Fig. 7a) and in the patients from 27% to 90% (
Fig. 7e), depending on the path crossing distance. These high detection rates even with very limited fields may be accounted for by two factors: first, for the cases with
Display Formula\({d_{pc}} = + 2\) m or
Display Formula\( + 12\) m, the pedestrians passed in front of the participant and thus eventually entered the residual central field to be detected. The detection RTs for these cases were therefore expected to be longer, which is exactly what our results of detection RT indicate (
Figs. 7b,
7f). Second, some participants may adopt a scanning strategy, given the regularity of the task and additionally for normally sighted subjects the visibility of the field obstruction. The simulated PFL also led to reduced decision RTs (
Fig. 7d) compared with the NV condition; and the patients showed the shortest decision RTs (
Fig. 6c). It is likely that the participants spent a longer time on detection; therefore, by the time the pedestrian was detected, the collision/noncollision event was so imminent that it required little time to judge and respond.