The sudden appearance of visual targets or any other features of interest in the peripheral visual field stimulates a cascade of events starting with a change in retinal activity. If not suppressed, this can eventually lead to ballistic eye movements known as saccadic eye movements (SEM).
1 Parasol cells, which are a subset of retinal ganglion cells (RGCs), provide input for this cascade of events.
2 Spatial information from the retina is subsequently encoded in a saccade generation network located in the cerebral cortex, thalamus, basal ganglia, cerebellum, superior colliculus (SC), and brainstem areas that maintain the spatial coding of the target with respect to the fovea.
3,4 This complex circuit then activates extra ocular motor neurons to break fixation of the current target of interest and to make adequate SEM to align the fovea with the new visual target of interest.
5
Eye tracking technology offers several methods for the qualitative (i.e., visual inspection) and quantitative evaluation (i.e., calculate saccadic properties) of SEM. Important parameters are saccadic reaction time (SRT), saccade velocity, amplitude, and duration.
6 Various studies have reported alterations in SEM parameters in patients on psychotropic drugs and in various neurologic diseases, such as Parkinson's disease, Alzheimer's disease, as well as in optic nerve pathologies and glaucoma.
6–12 This change in ocular dynamics led to the use of saccadic parameters as a marker for evaluating the integrity of saccade-generating neural network and in diagnosis of neurodegenerative conditions.
6,7
In previous studies, SEM parameters and the extent of saccade disruption were evaluated in patients with glaucomatous optic neuropathy. Kanjee et al.
6 evaluated glaucoma patients using a prosaccade step task, whereas Lamirel et al.
7 investigated patients with primary open-angle glaucoma (POAG) using static and kinetic targets. These studies reported significantly prolonged SRT and decreased eye movement precision in glaucoma patients. Smith et al.
8 and Asfaw et al.
9 found that the saccades and the spread of fixation during visual search processes were reduced in glaucoma patients when compared with their age-matched controls. Crabb et al.
10 observed characteristic eye movement patterns in glaucoma patients when viewing a driving scene in a hazard perception test (HPT). Their results showed that saccadic behavior was related to visual function and that patients with severe visual field defects showed fewer saccades per second than age-matched controls.
Investigators have also included SEM in visual field testing, so-called eye movement perimetry (EMP). During conventional visual field testing, such as in standard automated perimetry (SAP), a steady fixation throughout the course of testing is required. Especially the necessity to suppress reflexive eye movements compromises the test reliability.
11,12 Kim et al.
11 proposed an EMP system for visual field plotting based on eye movements as an alternative for SAP by presenting stimuli of various intensity levels (minimum of 15 dB). The visual field was reported on the basis of the minimum stimulus intensity seen (in dB). When compared between EMP and SAP, they reported less than 4 dB of sensitivity threshold difference in 92.8% of healthy subjects and 81.1% of glaucoma subjects.
11 The eye movements, however, were observed by the investigator using a video-based eye tracker and a decision algorithm classified each response as seen or not seen. Murray et al.
12 included remote eye tracking technology to quantify visual fields on the basis of primary eye movement responses toward the peripheral stimuli named ‘saccadic vector optokinetic perimetry' (SVOP) in both children and adults. They reported good agreement in discriminating normal eyes (adults: 99.2%, children: 99.1%) and eyes with glaucomatous visual field defects (adults 89.8%) when compared between the SVOP 41 test points and the C-40 screening test of Humphrey Field Analyser (HFA).
12 The EMP and SVOP were reported to be consistent in discriminating between normal and glaucoma when compared with the SAP. It showed the potential for assessing the extent of the visual field, even though it was only based on binary responses from the subjects (i.e., seen or unseen).
11,12 Previous investigations conducted by the current study group attempted to quantify some of the SEM characteristics obtained from a similar remote eye-tracking EMP system. A decision algorithm to classify an eye movement response as seen or unseen was included along with determining SRT for each seen point. This was denoted as a quantitative measure of visual field responsiveness.
13–16 A significant delay in SRT was found in mild, moderate, and severe glaucoma patients when compared with their age-matched controls,
13 indicating the potential importance of altered SEM values in glaucoma.
Several studies have examined the effects of factors, such as stimulus eccentricity, contrast, luminance, size, and age on SEM in isolation. Munoz et al.
17 reported age-related changes in performance of healthy human subjects during pro- and antisaccade task by projecting eccentric targets at 20° to either side of the fixation. They described the presence of delayed SRT and longer saccade duration in elderly subjects (60–79 years of age) in comparison to the younger age groups.
17 However, the effect of eccentricity and contrast was not explored. In another study, Pel et al.
14 investigated the repeatability and variability of SRT at locations that covered 60° horizontal and 40° vertical visual field. They reported good repeatability across three measurement series (on average the differences were within 100 ms) and significantly delayed SRT with lower stimulus contrast and increasing stimulus eccentricity, but the subject's age was not included as a factor in the mixed linear analysis.
14 Although the dependency of SRT on several factors, such as the age of the subject, stimulus intensities, and locations, are well documented in the literature, their interactions (including sex) and combined effect on SRT obtained at locations in a visual field test have not been reported. To use SRT as a functional marker in visual field testing, it is essential to address its variability in healthy subjects. Therefore, the current study aims to assess the interaction of age, sex, intensity, and eccentricity on SRT in healthy subjects using a mixed-model statistical analysis. The obtained data may serve as a first normative guide for EMP.