New locations added by the ARREST approach yield additional spatial information regarding visual field loss. Here, we demonstrate the feasibility of the approach in a cross-sectional study, hence the number of new locations added was for a single ARREST test. As demonstrated in
Figure 5, if participants are followed in future visits, ARREST adds more locations at each test visit, providing the clinician with increased information about the spatial nature of the person's visual field.
In our previous computer simulation, we used a bimodal ZEST 24-2 procedure, which did not include a growth pattern for either the ARREST procedure or the ZEST reference. The application of growth patterns reduce test time but can increase bias in estimates.
23,35 In our previous simulation study, ARREST used 20% to 40% fewer stimulus presentations once visual field loss became relatively advanced compared to ZEST.
3 The ZEST procedure utilized was that implemented as the default ZEST in the OPI, which does not incorporate a growth pattern, and does not cap presentation counts per location, hence can be quite lengthy. In this study, to compare ARREST against a procedure designed to represent a clinical standard, we used cZEST where we did incorporate a growth pattern (for cZEST but not for ARREST), and capped the maximum number of presentations per location at 12. This resulted in the total number of stimulus presentations being similar for both the procedures with the key benefit of ARREST being the addition of new test locations.
Performing a single visual field test using ARREST took an average of 8 minutes and 23 seconds. In this study, we did not try to optimize test duration, but instead a priori selected a desired number of 250 presentations. It is likely that the addition of a growth pattern, such as used in cZEST, would reduce the number of presentations required for ARREST, as would capping the number of permitted presentations per location. Adding a growth pattern to the underlying ZEST is simple (as in previous approaches), but care would need to be taken in subsequent tests to allow for any yellow or orange locations, or any additional locations in the field. The most beneficial method for spatial neighborhood logic when additional locations are added requires further exploration. Furthermore, there are various methods that are currently implemented in commercial perimeters, such as skipping false positives checks, not performing blind spot checks, or having dynamic response windows that significantly alter test duration.
36
We designed this study to demonstrate that the ARREST approach can be used to gain more spatial information without increasing the test duration of the underlying test algorithm (in this case, cZEST). It should be noted that ARREST can be applied to any underlying procedure as a layer on top of whatever “bells and whistles” have been incorporated within the underlying commercial procedure to save test time. By way of an example, the ARREST approach of not fully thresholding locations with visual field sensitivity less than 17 dB, nor testing established perimetrically blind locations, could be added to already highly abbreviated test strategies, such as SITA-Faster
37 to allow individualized increased spatial resolution.
The key feature of ARREST is that it relies on information collected during preceding visual field tests from the same individual. Prior data is used to not only decide which locations require full assessment but also to determine where new added locations should be placed in the visual field. Furthermore, the determination of how many additional visual field points to add is also based on a conservative calculation of likely total test presentation numbers derived from the prior test result. For the implementation of ARREST explored herein, we simply use a relatively standard ZEST procedure as the underlying procedure. However, as noted above, ARREST can be applied to any test procedure, including those that use prior information to potentially speed up the test. For example, we have previously shown that information from prior visual fields can be used to bias the prior probability distribution for ZEST
38 or combined with suprathreshold testing strategies.
39 Depending on the preferred trade-off between test speed and spatial information gain, utilizing the prior visual field information in additionally sophisticated ways could result in either shorter tests, or increased spatial sampling. Further work is required to explore these trade-offs in detail, in particular, exploring the ability to both detect progression and also optimally assess areas of visual field important for tasks of daily living. Our current implementation of ARREST simply chooses the steepest gradient in the visual field as the region of interest for new test locations, however, this could also be biased by either prior information (for example, to add locations in an area different to the areas that have received additional locations in recent tests) or weighted somehow by relative importance to visual function, depending on what has been discovered at prior test visits.
The number of locations added by ARREST can vary markedly for eyes with advanced visual field damage for early tests in a testing sequence. For example,
Figure 6 shows two participants with advanced field loss (cZEST MTD = −28.47 dB in participant E and −19.50 dB in participant F). ARREST added 15 additional locations for participant E (
Fig. 6 top right panel) and 0 new locations for participant F (
Fig. 6 lower right panel). The number of locations to be added was determined based on the first test in the series (left panels for participants E and F in
Fig. 6). We did not collect additional tests for these two participants, however, we determined that if participants E and F were to undergo another visit, the ARREST approach would add 15 and 11 additional locations respectively while maintaining similar test duration as previous. Thus, our cross-sectional study represents seeing a new patient with existing field damage for their first two visual fields, but in a typical clinical scenario, where someone may be followed with regular visits over many years, the ARREST approach will add many additional locations compared with the results in this study.
Global visual field indices lack spatial information but are commonly used as outcome measures from clinical trials for disease staging and risk calculation.
6,29 In this study, we showed that ARREST fields can be interpreted to obtain global measures (MTD). This result implies that global indices for ARREST could be used across longitudinally acquired visual field data, however the best way to do this requires further study, particularly by simulation of the performance of ARREST on longitudinal data series. Because ARREST adds locations along the borders of scotoma, these new locations may mildly shift the MTD from one visit to the next through the addition of either more normal or damaged points. We account for this, in part, by spatially weighting our MTD index. The effect of adding locations on the MTD in our case examples is shown in
Figure 3 where the MTD is compared between ARREST 1 and ARREST 2. As noted earlier, the difference in MTD for cZEST and ARREST 1 is driven by the difference in the underlying ZEST procedures, rather than due to the ARREST framework per se. An alternate approach for calculating MTD that might be useful for progression could involve calculating the resultant MTD if all remaining possible visual field locations are interpolated at each visit from the existing measured locations. We note that computing global indices somewhat defeats the purpose of adding spatial information with ARREST, but is entirely feasible.
Previously, studies have shown that censoring threshold values below 20 dB had very little effect on glaucomatous progression assessment for either point-wise or censored MD trend analysis.
17,40 As concurrent change might occur in neighboring locations in moderate and advanced visual field loss, those studies have recommended shifting the lower end of the perimetric dynamic range to 15 to 19 dB
40 and to redesign newer perimetric algorithms to test more useful locations.
41 ARREST provides a framework for doing this, however, it does not completely eliminate testing below 15 to 19 dB. ARREST checks for visual field progression from 16 dB to 0 dB (yellow to orange or red) using spot checks. In highly advanced visual field loss, progression to perimetrically blind (i.e. from yellow to orange or red in ARREST) may provide meaningful information. For locations with sensitivities above 17 dB, standard progression criteria that are currently applied to longitudinal series of visual fields can be applied to data from ARREST (as described in our previous simulation study of progression analysis).
3
Adding spatial locations complicates the analytical determination of visual field progression, however, our previous simulation study
3 explores solutions and demonstrates feasibility. Specifically, we have shown that event-based criteria that consolidates pointwise information work well. There are many event-based criteria that can be used,
42 however, all share four parameters: (1) a definition of baseline from which progression must occur, (2) a number of visual field points that must show a decrease in sensitivity from baseline, (3) the level of dB decrease that is considered important, and (4) the number of visits in a row where the decrease in sensitivity must be confirmed. Our previous simulation study demonstrates that this approach can be successfully implemented, with each new location requiring the relevant number of test repeats prior to consideration for progression.
Our current implementation of ARREST only differs from its underlying procedure when at least one location has sensitivity < 17 dB, and there is estimated to be enough presentation budget (aiming for approximately 250 presentations in total) to allow additional test locations to be added. One outcome of this approach is that entirely “clean” visual fields terminate in fewer presentations (approximately 210 on average). Hence, depending on the desired test duration, it may be possible to add test locations in situations of earlier visual field loss than the requisite 17 dB cutoff described here. For example, additional locations of known higher risk for glaucomatous damage in the macular area could be added,
43–45 or additional locations could be added in areas surrounding established scotomata where the sensitivity has not quite reached the 17 dB criterion. This may have particular benefit in exploring the spatial distribution of visual field loss close to fixation, in otherwise relatively normal fields. Note, whereas we have tested people with glaucoma in this study, ARREST is not a visual field procedure specific for glaucoma but can be used to explore the spatial nature of visual field defects in any condition. Further simulation work using empirical visual field series containing both 10-2 and 24-2 data may be helpful to explore the various trade-offs prior to recommending a more sophisticated approach.
In summary, here, we present a feasibility study of ARREST in a sample of people with glaucoma. The study demonstrates the feasibility of the ARREST approach applied to an underlying ZEST procedure; however, the approach can be applied to any extant visual field testing algorithm. We collected normative reference data here to determine TD for our laboratory algorithms implemented using the OPI, however, the ARREST framework applied to existing procedures would be able to utilize existing normative data. The ARREST framework has no features that are specifically designed to maximize performance for glaucoma. Hence, the principals involved in ARREST should be able to be applied for visual field testing of neurological defects, peripheral visual field defects, and for macular damage, however, empirical study will be necessary. The current iteration of ARREST tested herein would, however, require minor modification to the threshold checking procedure for situations where significant visual field recovery may be expected, stroke for example. As demonstrated in our previous simulations and by the case series herein, ARREST is designed to progressively improve the spatial description of the visual field with increasing numbers of follow-up visits, in the absence of increasing test duration.