Among the tests we used in determining structure–function relationships, we found that the scotopic red test performed poorly, with a small prediction range. The scotopic cyan test performed better and just marginally worse than the mesopic test. However, the normative profile for the latter required more complicated modeling, due to the peculiar dropoff in sensitivity toward the fovea and its specific wavelength also required an adjustment for MP absorption. This is important, since MP concentration can vary across individuals and is reduced in patients with AMD,
29 creating differences in the intensity of light that reaches the retina. For our analysis, we derived our correction from an independent optical analysis of blue light absorption, which might not be available in all clinical contexts. The strongest relationship was seen with the mesopic test. This was likely due in part to its extended dynamic range toward lower sensitivities. A secondary analysis on locations with abnormal (thicker) RPE-E values revealed that most of the locations likely affected by drusen were correctly predicted to have lower sensitivities (
Fig. 4). These are the locations in which progression is more likely to be observed in a longitudinal follow-up, making this result particularly valuable, and this is noteworthy. This was also the case for the scotopic cyan test but not for the scotopic red test. In general, however, the predictive power of the structure–function models was small. This was unsurprising, since the sensitivity loss in patients with drusen (not necessarily iAMD) is expected to be small
15,16 and therefore very close to the range of test–retest variability. Indeed, the spread of residuals of the predictions in
Figure 4, at least for the mesopic test, was compatible with the 95% test–retest variability range reported by Welker et al.
15 and Pfau et al.
10 Hence, although more complex modeling, such as with machine learning,
36 might increase the accuracy of the prediction of functional loss, a large improvement is unlikely. Previous work reported better structure–function relationship in patients with AMD, but the results are not always directly comparable with ours and focused on mesopic tests. Querques et al.,
37 for example, used a clinical categorical classification of autofluorescence patterns and included eyes with geographic atrophy. Acton et al.
38 classified the tested locations based on the presence or absence of microperimetric defects and studied the structural differences between these two classes. Hence, in contrast to our work, the structural metric was used as the independent variable in their analysis. Work by Wu et al.
16 was more similar to ours and also reported a better predictive power of their structure–function model for mesopic tests. However, their model explicitly included age and eccentricity and predicted the raw sensitivity. Given the very small sensitivity loss in these patients, the parameters of age and eccentricity are likely to be stronger predictors than structural features. Moreover, the effect of eccentricity was modeled with separate regression equations, one for each location, on a limited number (five) of retinal positions from the fovea. Such modeling relied on the assumption that the same location in the perimetric grid was at the same eccentricity from the fovea across different tests. Such an assumption was not possible in our case, since we positioned the testing grid on the structural maps by matching the fundus pictures from the MAIA and the Spectralis acquisitions. Therefore, the position of the grid could vary from test to test in relation to the anatomic fovea. Finally, if eccentricity was instead included as a continuous variable, such an approach would have required very different structure–function models for each microperimetric test, because of the large differences in the effect of eccentricity (
Fig. 3). Instead, in our analysis, age and eccentricity were part of the normative models used as a reference to calculate the sensitivity loss in patients with drusen, so that the contribution of the structural features could be isolated. Therefore, we feel that our methodology yields results that are more reflective of the actual effect of structural changes in patients with drusen. Nevertheless, the modeling proposed by Wu et al.
16 would be extremely valuable if such limitations could be overcome by controlling the position of the perimetric grid. For example, OCT maps could be used to center the perimetric grid on the anatomic fovea across different tests and standardize its position on the retina. This could be the objective of future work.