Abstract
Purpose:
The purpose of this study was to determine if a battery of polarization-modulated stimuli, quantified as a single metric, is effective in identifying macular disease in the presence/absence of cataract or pseudophakia.
Methods:
Using a modified liquid crystal display, polarization pattern perception (PPP) for a formulated battery of geometric and logMAR stimuli was evaluated in participants that had either no eye pathology (healthy participants) or were grouped according to the presence of cataract, pseudophakia, and/or age-related macular degeneration (AMD). PPP was quantified as response frequencies to individual stimuli, and as a novel monocular polarization sensitivity score (Ps) based on perception of the stimulus battery set.
Results:
Stimulus response frequencies were pattern-dependent and, compared with healthy participants, reduced for cataract and AMD groups but not for subjects with pseudophakia. Compared with healthy eyes (n = 47, median Ps = 17), Ps was significantly reduced by AMD (n = 59, median Ps = 1, P < 0.001) and, to a lesser extent, by cataracts (n = 80, median Ps = 6, P < 0.001). There was no significant difference between Ps for healthy and pseudophakic eyes (n = 47, median Ps = 13, P = 0.323). There was no significant correlation between Ps and logMAR visual acuity.
Conclusions:
In the absence of significant cataract, or in pseudophakia, a set of polarization-modulated visual stimuli, quantified as the Ps score, distinguishes AMD from healthy maculae.
Translational Relevance:
Perception of polarization-modulated stimuli, previously shown to be macula-dependent in a laboratory setting, is effective as a test of macular function in health and disease in a clinic setting.
Humans can perceive and identify structured visual stimuli defined only by their state of polarization,
1–3 an ability that has been termed polarization pattern perception (PPP). Contrary to previous assumptions, evidence based on PPP suggests that humans are capable of a high degree of polarization sensitivity, being able to detect differences in angle of polarization as little as 5 degrees and differences in degrees of polarization of 25% or less.
As with the related phenomenon of Haidinger's brush (HB),
4–7 PPP is dependent on: (i) the ocular media transmitting linear polarized light without significant modification; (ii) the structural integrity of the fovea; and (iii) the presence of macular pigment. Any ocular disorder affecting one or more of these factors may interfere with polarization perception. This is known for HB, where the perception of the phenomenon is reduced or abolished by disorders of the retina and macula,
8–10 and/or by low levels of macular pigment.
11
Refractive error and media opacities have little effect on the visibility of HB,
9,12 the perception of which has been proposed as a prognostic indicator for cataract surgery.
12 However, the detection of HB is relatively nonspecific with respect to different eye conditions.
10 Furthermore, the quantification of polarization perception based on HB alone is limited by the spatiotemporal characteristics of this single percept. Other drawbacks to the use of HB in clinical practice include the difficulty with which the phenomenon is perceived, the tendency of the static percept to fade rapidly, and the cumbersome electromechanical apparatus required for its generation.
Our methodology for determining polarization pattern perception overcomes the disadvantages of previous clinical applications of HB in several ways. First, polarization-modulated patterned stimuli are easily generated by inexpensive, compact, solid-state technology. Second, in addition to qualitative assessments, quantitative polarization perceptual measures can be obtained using sets of polarization-modulated patterns that can be graded according to salience. Third, polarization pattern stimuli are easier to discern and describe than the peculiarities of HB.
Previous studies of human polarization perception typically used established psychophysical stimuli (e.g. gratings) or Landolt-C configurations, and employed trained observers.
2,3 The stimuli of the present study were developed for naïve observers in a clinic setting, and were designed to provide a stimulus set of graded salience that yielded a robust quantification of polarization pattern perception. The stimuli included those successfully used in previous psychophysical and electrophysiological investigations of human vision (e.g. gratings and checkerboard patterns), novel geometric patterns whose spatial discontinuities/sharp edges complemented the underlying radial nature of macular architecture and optotypes.
In a previous report,
13 we demonstrated a graded set of stimuli that were appropriate to assess the full extent of polarization perception in normally sighted individuals, and that normal sensitivity measures were reduced in a heterogeneous set of individuals with clinically unhealthy eyes. Our principal goal in this study was to determine whether polarization-modulated stimuli are effective in distinguishing individuals with macular disease, as represented by age-related macular degeneration (AMD), from individuals with healthy maculae. To do so, we assessed individuals diagnosed with two eye disorders (AMD and/or cataract) and one surgical intervention (cataract surgery). The reasons for choosing these particular eye conditions are: (i) all are common in individuals over 65 years of age; (ii) AMD and cataract frequently co-exist, so the effect of each and both combined on PPP must be understood if the latter is to be of diagnostic value; (iii) intraocular lens implants made of polymer materials could potentially alter the polarization state of transmitted light, again interfering with the diagnostic value of PPP.
Available data for the patient demographic described above indicates that, whereas cataracts and AMD co-exist in many individuals (10% of individuals undergoing cataract surgery have co-existing AMD, Day et al. 2015), the majority of individuals with AMD will either have visually insignificant cataracts or will be pseudophakic. Furthermore, the possible acceleration of AMD following cataract surgery (e.g. Beaver Dam study
14), although disputed,
15 emphasizes the importance of postsurgical macular function assessment and monitoring.
A novel metric of polarization pattern perception, based on the weighted sum of the polarization responses to each pattern/optotype, was used to derive a polarization sensitivity score (Ps), which was used to compare polarization pattern responses in the clinically defined categories. In particular, we sought to determine: (i) whether a quantifiable polarization-modulated stimulus set, generated with existing LCD technology, is effective in distinguishing AMD from healthy maculae; (ii) the effects of pseudophakia on PPP; and (iii) if, as with HB, cataractous media opacities have little effect on PPP.
Of the 221 participants (data summary in
Supplementary Material Table S1), 21 had no manifest pathology in either eye (healthy group). The patient group comprised 200 individuals who, in one or both eyes, were normophakic, pseudophakic, or had cataract, and either had AMD or clinically healthy fundi. Cataracts were defined as any symptomatic lens opacification identifiable on slit-lamp examination. AMD was defined by an Age-Related Eye Disease Study (AREDS) grading of two or greater.
16
All healthy participants were over 60 years of age, chosen randomly from a large database to affect a group-mean age within one decade of the patient group. Individuals with single or multiple pathologies – other than cataract, pseudophakia, or AMD – were excluded from the study, as were data from eyes with visual acuities worse than 1.2 logMAR.
The healthy-eye diagnostic group comprised healthy-eye data from the patient group combined with data of one eye (randomly chosen) from each individual in the healthy group (
n = 47; normophakia / no AMD;
Table 1). The unhealthy-eye diagnostic groups (see
Table 1) comprised data from one or both eyes of normophakic individuals with AMD, and those with cataract or pseudophakia in the presence or absence of AMD. Different uniocular pathologies and asymmetry of bilateral pathology justified the separate analysis of eye pairs.
Table 1. Eye Diagnostic Categories (332 Eyes of 221 Participants), Grouped According to the Presence or Absence of Cataract or Pseudophakia
Table 1. Eye Diagnostic Categories (332 Eyes of 221 Participants), Grouped According to the Presence or Absence of Cataract or Pseudophakia
All patients with pseudophakia had uneventful cataract surgery at least 4 months prior to testing. Of the 140 pseudophakic eyes, 63 had TECNIS iTEC PCB00 intraocular lenses (range = 6–34D, mean = 22.3, SD = 4.3), and 35 had Alcon AcrySof MA60AC intraocular lenses (range = 13–27.5, mean = 22.2, SD = 2.6). The intraocular lens data of 42 eyes were unknown.
Corrected distance visual acuity was measured using a standard Bailey-Lovie (early treatment diabetic retinopathy study [ETDRS]) chart, and recorded as a logMAR value. Optical coherence tomography (OCT) was performed using a Topcon DRI OCT Triton Plus swept source system. Allocation to diagnostic categories was based on clinical assessment, including slit-lamp examination, dilated fundoscopy, and OCT.
PPP was assessed using hardware and methodology detailed elsewhere.
13 In brief, participants were asked to identify images presented on a polarization stimulus generator that consisted of a liquid crystal display (Asus VS278H from ASUSTeK Computer Inc, Taiwan) from which the front polarizer had been removed.
1–3,17 The screen emitted a constant polarization-independent luminance of 8.0 cd m
−2 with a peak wavelength 460 nm, and subtended visual angles of 11 degrees (width) by 6.5 degrees (height) at an observation distance of 3 m. Polarization output was calibrated as described elsewhere,
3 and confirmed to be predominantly linearly polarized (degree of linear polarization = 0.94). Polarization
E-vector axes, measured anticlockwise from the horizontal, varied from 54 degrees for greyscale 000 (foreground of stimulus images) to 147 degrees for greyscale 255 (stimulus background).
Ametropic participants were corrected for the 3 m working distance using optically isotropic (stress-free) glass lenses mounted in a trial frame. All testing was monocular. Each eye of a participant was assessed in turn by one of two trained ophthalmic technicians, both of whom were unaware of the participant's clinical details. Before testing, the technician explained the task and the expected appearance of the stimuli according to a set preamble. Typical run-times for the full series of slides were between 5 and 10 minutes per eye.
The stimulus set consisted of uniform fields, geometric patterns and optotypes (
Fig. 1).
13 The uniform field stimuli comprised either: (i) a blank screen with a grey scale of constant 000 (foreground “black,” angle of polarization 54 degrees) to give a static HB percept, or (ii) a greyscale 000 alternating with 255 (background “white,” angle of polarization 147 degrees) at 1 Hz to give an alternating (dynamic) HB percept. Pattern stimuli, composed of foreground and background polarization states, consisted of one of six geometric patterns (see
Fig. 1). The checkerboard was presented either as a pattern-reversing 1 cycle per degree (cpd) image, or as a static image with a fundamental spatial frequency of 0.25, 2, 4, 6, 9, or 12 cpd. Standard Sloan optotypes were presented in random order of five optotypes per screen of a particular logMAR value. The range covered was logMAR 0.3–1.2 in 0.1 increments.
Two response criteria for polarization pattern sensitivity were used. First, pattern/optotype identification, defined as the ability to identify accurately the stimulus pattern/optotype. Second, pattern detection, defined as the ability to detect but not identify the presence of a pattern.
For the second part of the study, the collected responses from an individual to the polarization stimulus set was represented as a single metric quantifying that individual's ability to perceive polarization stimuli. The polarization sensitivity (
Ps) score metric devised for the present study was the weighted sum of scores of the stimulus set response for individual participants. The
Ps score was based on previous findings,
13 and relates to the relative sensitivity of the criteria of detection and identification of stimuli. A score of 2 was given for the correct identification of each of the 14 geometric or 35 optotype stimuli; a score of one was given for each detection (without identification) of a pattern stimulus; a score of zero was given for the inability to detect a pattern stimulus from its background, or incorrect optotype identification. The maximum possible
Ps score per eye was 98.
The authors thank the following: Alison Marriot (Departmental Manager, Warwick Hospital) and Sara Queen (Clinical Director, Eye Clinic, Warwick Hospital) for administrative and clinical assistance; Mark Dunne (Aston University, UK) for assistance with statistical analyses; and Shelby E Temple for critical review of the manuscript.
G.P.M. is partly funded by a grant from the European Society of Cataract and Refractive Surgeons.
Author Contributions: G.P.M. conceived the idea, built the equipment, designed the experiments, conducted the data analysis, wrote the paper, and was the project administrator. S.J.A. did the psychophysical analysis, designed the experiments, conducted the data analysis, and wrote the paper. R.A.A. gathered the statistics, conducted the data analysis, and reviewed the paper. M.G. conducted the data acquisition, conducted the database management, and reviewed the paper. D.R. conducted the data acquisition, conducted the database management, and reviewed the paper.
Disclosure: G.P. Misson, None; S.J. Anderson, None; R.A. Armstrong, None; M. Gilett, None; D. Reynolds, None