We used a live OKN detection and an adaptive psychometric method to measure CS by searching for the contrast threshold at which the OKN just occurs for a given spatial frequency. As an adaptive psychometric method, we used the QUEST+ algorithm, because this procedure was advised to be used in CS testing.
16 We implemented the algorithm in its one-dimensional form for the contrast level management; however, the implementation could be extended by using a multidimensional psychometric function on spatial or temporal frequency to increase the speed of the assessment of the spatiotemporal CSF. The OKN detection performance of the newly proposed method showed sufficient accuracy compared to the subjective judgment and very similar performance to the offline algorithmic-based analysis, therefore allowing the implementation of an adaptive psychometric procedure. However, not all trials were detected correctly. Some trials were found to contain a robust OKN response in the correct direction but have not been detected by the proposed algorithm, or vice versa. A possible reason for this is the limitation of not having a blink identification in the live OKN detection procedure. A second limitation is that the noise level has been estimated across a quite short time period, because the time performance of the measurements was privileged over an extended detection time. Third, some trails, especially those in which a low-contrast-grating was presented, may have contained OKN-SPs of a velocity below the defined threshold, whereas the experienced observer recognized it still as a valid OKN-SP. Such algorithmic misjudgment may have happened because the detection algorithms used a fixed threshold value for the OKN-SP velocity. As already shown in the zebrafish experiment by Rinner et al.,
33 the gain of OKN (ratio of the OKN-SP velocity to the physical velocity of the stimulus) varies across contrast levels and is lower for low-contrast stimuli. Hence, we suggest instead thresholding the OKN-SP according to the contrast level, which could be implemented in the future. Nonetheless, the method of OKN detection we used in the current study was found to be superior to previous works, because the false-positive rate of existing detection algorithms was judged as too high for the implementation of adaptive methods.
1 The CS value was obtained from the Weibull fit, already used in the previous research,
5 because the inverted value of the contrast level at which the OKN response was expected to occur with a probability of 50 %. Furthermore, the assessment of the log-parabolic fit, which was already proposed in the previous work by Lesmes et al.,
31 showed robust goodness of fit in the trend of CS values over selected spatial frequencies. Here, the CSF's peak is shifted toward smaller spatial frequencies, compared to the CS curves obtained in clinical practice in which nonmoving stimuli are used. However, this effect has already been observed in the previous study.
34 Moreover, the current study results show an agreement of the CSF-peak placement with the previous study by Burr et al.,
34 considering the stimulus velocity used in each experiment. As a next point, the current study aimed to measure CS under healthy and blurred vision conditions in several steps, and thus replicate the known effect of decreasing CS with increasing defocus,
18–20 mainly having an impact on gratings of higher spatial frequencies. Such testing is possibly done in two ways, once by decreasing vision to a certain value of visual performance, as done by Marmor et al.,
18 or by using an optical power of a lens resulting in comparable refractive error (defocus) across subjects.
32 Since the current study tested emmetropes, lenses of constant values have been used over all participants to artificially worsen their vision. Here, the defocus levels have been chosen to induce the desired blur, although with negligible simultaneous magnification-induced effects on the presented spatial frequencies.
29 Hence, lenses in the range from +1.5 D to +2.5 D in +0.5 D steps were used. Because the current study targeted clinical testing, similar to previous work,
32 we did not use cycloplegic agents to suppress accommodation. Furthermore, we avoided cycloplegia because these substances result in pupil dilation, leading to increasing high-order aberrations and decreasing the CS. As the results show decreasing CS to increasing defocus, mainly for higher spatial frequencies, the current study successfully replicated the effect of defocus on CS in OKN-based measurements. Possible reasons for the variation in the impact of defocus are the residual refractive error of the emmetropic participants, their status of high-order aberrations, or differences in lags of accommodations, because these factors are highly individual.
35,36 At the last point, since the current study used a square-wave grating to enable displaying also gratings of high spatial frequencies, the effect of higher harmonics might be comprised in the data. As shown by Campbell et al.
37 and Graham et al.,
38 based on Fourier theory the detection threshold for a mixture of sine-waves is the determined by the component that reaches its contrast threshold first. In our case, for a square-wave of a base frequency
SF = 1.5 cpd or higher, the first-order harmonic (three times the base frequency) is
SF = 4.5 cpd or higher. Given that the detection thresholds for this spatial frequencies are beyond the peak of the CSF, only the higher harmonics of low-spatial-frequency square-wave gratings might be considered as relevant. Nonetheless, because no subject showed a peak CS that was three times higher than the one at about
SF = 0.7 cpd in the previous study using a sine-wave grating,
34 it appears that the participants detected the square wave gratings by its fundamental
SF and not by one of the higher harmonics. In summary we consider the effect of higher harmonics on our OKN-based CS in our selected range of spatial frequencies to be negligible.