Ethical approval for this study was obtained from the UCL Research Ethics Committee, and all procedures adhered to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki. Two experienced psychophysical observers (observer A, aged 31 years; observer B, aged 41 years) and one naive observer (observer C, aged 29 years) undertook the acuity tasks. Participants had no significant ocular abnormalities and a VA of 6/5 or better. Refractive error was corrected prior to the start of each testing session using trial lenses for foveal (observer A, −0.25 diopters sphere (DS); observer B, −0.75 DS/−0.25 diopters cylinder (DC) × 100; and observer C, −3.75 DS/−0.50 DC × 180) and extrafoveal testing at 10° eccentricity in the nasal field of the right eye (observer A, plano; observer B, −0.50 DS/−0.25 DC × 100; and observer C, −3.50 DS/−0.50 DC × 180).
VO and conventional letters had a conventional 5:1 size/stroke ratio and were generated using MATLAB (version 7.6; MathWorks, Inc., Natick, MA, USA). Experiments were controlled by an Apple Macintosh computer (Apple, Inc., Cupertino, CA, USA) with stimuli displayed on a γ-corrected high-resolution (1280 × 1024 pixels) Dell Trinitron P992 CRT monitor (Dell Corp. Ltd, Bracknell, Berkshire, UK). True 14-bit contrast resolution was achieved using a Bits++ video processor (Cambridge Research Systems, Ltd., Rochester, UK), and spatial scaling of the stimuli was done using the OpenGL capabilities of the computer's built-in graphics card (ATI Radeon X1600; AMD, Sunnyvale, CA, USA). Following previous work,
41 VO stimuli of a pseudo-high-pass design were constructed with an inner black core flanked by a white border (of 106.6 cd/m
2) half the width of the central section (
Fig. 1), yielding a Michelson contrast of 98%. Vanishing stimuli were presented on a gray background whose luminance (53.3 cd/m
2) matched the mean luminance of the letter. For the conventional black-on-white letters, the white background had a luminance of 113.2 cd/m
2, again yielding a high contrast of 99%. Stimuli were presented for 500 ms. All testing was conducted under low room illumination to avoid screen reflections with a viewing distance of 8 m for all foveal testing and 1.6 m for all peripheral testing. The screen subtended 11.6° × 9.8° and one pixel subtended 0.55 arc min at the closer distance.
Recognition threshold VA was determined using an adaptive staircase procedure (QUEST) for the right eye of each participant, for both conventional and VOs, in the fovea and periphery. Our rationale for testing only one eye of participants was based upon two considerations: (1) the relative interocular difference (or lack thereof) in recognition acuity levels in the participants examined and (2) limiting participant fatigue. Participants performed a 10 alternative forced-choice (AFC) (Sloan letter set) task, reporting the identity of the optotype presented. The initial letter size displayed was 115.8 × 115.8 arc min. The slope (β) of the psychometric function used was set to 3.5 with gamma (guess rate) and pThreshold parameters set to 10% and 75% correct, respectively. Each test run involved 50 letter presentations in total with the final acuity estimate determined from QUEST's built-in maximum likelihood estimation procedure of threshold. Participants were made aware of the letter set available, and the participant’s verbal report of the letter identity was entered by the examiner on the keyboard.
These measurements were repeated three times for six different levels of induced stray light (where order of presentation of levels was randomized). Differing levels of induced stray light were created using either one of five white resin opacity-containing filters (filters 1 to 5 in increasing density; LEE Fog Filters, Andover, UK) or with no filter (filter 0). This manipulation was intended to simulate known increases in light scatter and absorption with age (but without the associated and individually variable loss of vision attributable to reduced neural function). A computer-controlled stray-light meter (C-Quant; Oculus GmbH) was used to measure the baseline intraocular stray light (no fog filter), using the psychophysical compensation comparison method
42 and the individual increase in forward intraocular stray light when each of the filters was placed in front of the eye close to the cornea. Values are expressed as log [stray-light parameter] (log[s]), with higher values indicating greater levels of stray light.
Figure 2 demonstrates the increase in stray-light parameter with each increasing grade of filter for an experienced participant (observer A) and a participant naive to psychophysical tests of this nature (observer C). Baseline measures with no filter were within the normal expected range given the age of the participants, and it can be seen that the filters progressively increase the stray-light value as expected. A measure of the reliability of the stray-light value is provided by the C-Quant, and all measurements were found to be within acceptable reliability parameters with expected SD ≤0.08 log units and reliability coefficient (Q) ≥1. Using its normative database, the C-Quant software permits an estimation of the typical age increase that is simulated with each WOF. Filters 1, 2, and 3 increase the stray-light levels of an average 31-year-old (observer A) to that of a 62-, 72-, and 90-year-old, respectively. The last two filters (4 and 5) take the stray-light value into levels expected with significant cataract.
16