Figure 3 shows baseline visual parameters as a function of the age group, and
Table 1 shows the characteristics of our sample (see
Supplementary Material S1 for the complete data set). All participants had scores of 27 or higher on the MMSE. As expected, the older participants had significantly higher levels of intraocular light scatter measured by the C-Quant than the younger participants. C-Quant data from five older individuals were removed from the analysis because their values did not meet the quality parameters. In addition, binocular logMAR values and contrast thresholds were significantly elevated in the older compared to the younger participants (see
Figs. 3B,
3C,
3D). The results of the independent
t-tests between groups are provided in
Figure 3. A 2-way ANOVA showed a main effect of age on contrast sensitivity:
F(1,76) = 31.00,
P < 0.001 and eccentricity:
F(1,76) = 77.95,
P < 0.001 but no significant interaction between age and eccentricity:
F(1,76) = 0.23,
P = 0.63.
As illustrated in
Figure 4, there was no main effect of glare for any of the motion tasks (motion contrast:
F(1,76) = 0.53,
P = 0.47; translational global motion:
F(1,76) = 2.33,
P = 0.13; and biological motion:
F(1,76) = 1.86,
P = 0.18), nor for the levels of suppression (no main effect of glare on SI:
F(1,76) = 1.16,
P = 0.29). There was also no significant interaction between age and glare for motion contrast (
F(1,76) = 0.16,
P = 0.69) or biological motion (
F(1,76) = 1.05,
P = 0.31), nor for the levels of suppression (no significant interaction between age and glare for the SI:
F(1,76) = 0.09,
P = 0.77).
Regarding eccentricity, motion thresholds were elevated in peripheral vision (main effect of eccentricity on motion contrast: F(1,76) = 141.32, P < 0.001; translational global motion: F(1,76) = 10.72, P < 0.01, and biological motion: F(1,76) = 294.81, P < 0.001). There was a main effect of eccentricity on the SI (F(1,76) = 9.23, P < 0.01), and this effect varied with age group. Specifically, compared with younger adults, older adults exhibited reduced SIs centrally but increased peripherally (F(1,76) = 29.05, P < 0.001). The presence of glare did not exacerbate these eccentricity effects (no significant interactions between glare and eccentricity for motion contrast: F(1,76) = 1.35, P = 0.25; translational global motion: F(1,76) = 0.02, P = 0.90; biological motion: F(1,76) = 0.20, P = 0.66, and the SI: F(1,76) = 0.17, P = 0.68).
We also explored whether our measured straylight values in both groups correlated with motion perception thresholds in central vision for the glare condition.
Figure 5 and
Table 2 present the results of this analysis, showing that the 2 motion tasks whose thresholds were significantly correlated with the straylight estimate (after correcting for multiple comparisons, resulting in a
P value of 0.01 as statistically significant) were motion contrast (
r = 0.47,
P = 0.005, 95% confidence intervals = 0.26 to 0.66), and biological motion (
r = 0.55,
P < 0.001, 95% confidence intervals = 0.27 to 0.75).