In these two experiments, we found that performing automated perimetry with a moving stimulus gave higher sensitivities and lower variability than a static stimulus, when all other aspects of the testing were kept the same including the instrument, testing paradigm, and test algorithm. This both extended the dynamic range into locations with more advanced disease (experiment 1), and reduced test–retest variability while within the dynamic range (experiment 2).
Most subjects in the study reported preferring testing with the moving stimulus. This preference strengthened, from 42% on the first visit to 62% on the second visit, suggesting that it is not purely due to the novelty of the test. One reason for this preference may be gleaned from the psychometric functions measured in experiment 1. The static stimulus resulted in wider interquartile ranges at most locations. This factor increases the range of stimuli, and hence the proportion of stimulus presentations, over which the subject is unsure whether they saw the stimulus or not. That subjective uncertainty can be stressful for the subject, leading them to prefer the moving stimulus test which causes them less uncertainty. The stress would also be expected to increase fatigue, and hence increase variability.
42 Perimetry has always been endured rather than enjoyed by patients
43; even a modest improvement in the patient experience would be welcomed by many, and it may also improve the diagnostic information available to clinicians.
Testing for both experiments was conducted on an Octopus perimeter, controlled via the Open Perimetry Interface.
35 Using a clinical instrument, and the same seen/not seen testing paradigm as in clinical perimetry means that the test is more familiar to subjects, which would be expected to allow more reliable results; in addition, the conclusions are more immediately translatable to clinical care. However, testing relied on the current firmware programmed into the Octopus perimeter. Notably, this means that, for the moving stimulus, the response window during which the subject's responses (in the form of button pushes) were recorded only lasted for the time when the stimulus was present, because the firmware treats it as a kinetic perimetry stimulus. By contrast for the static stimulus, the response window extended 500 ms after the stimulus turned off. This will have caused some valid responses to be missed for the moving stimulus, especially near the detection threshold where the off detection pathway may produce a larger neural response than the on pathway.
21 A consequence is that sensitivities were likely underestimated for some locations using the moving stimulus. Indeed, on
Figure 4 is it seen that the moving stimulus seemed to produce lower sensitivities than the static stimulus when greater than 31 dB, probably owing to this caveat. It is possible that the test–retest variability for the moving stimulus shown in
Figure 5 is also an overestimate for the same reason, and hence that the benefits of using a moving stimulus in clinical perimetry are being underestimated.
A consequence of this requirement to use a 500-ms stimulus is that small movements in fixation during the presentation would be expected. As in most current clinical perimeters, individual stimulus presentations were not discarded based on fixation instability. Instead, fixation was closely monitored by the technician to ensure that the instability was not excessive. It should be noted that this caveat applies equally to the moving and static stimuli, and there is not reason to expect fixation instability to differ significantly between the stimuli, especially because the order of testing was randomized.
The trajectory of the moving stimulus was a straight line, parallel to the average nerve fiber bundle orientation at that location, according to the map of Jansonius et al.
26 That map was derived by tracing nerve fiber bundles on 27 deidentified fundus photos, with no adjustments for factors such as axial length that could influence the trajectories, or factors such as age and media opacity that could have selectively influenced visibility of the bundles. Their map was also spatially limited owing to the visibility of the bundles, and several test locations fall within regions at which the trajectories were extrapolated beyond the observed range of traced bundles, and/or in the nasal region at which the model is undefined. Finally, the map adjusted for the position of the optic nerve head relative to the horizontal midline, which was not taken into account here. An optimal implementation of the moving stimulus technique could instead individualize the stimulus trajectories based on the observed nerve fiber bundle orientations in that particular eye. However, there are significant obstacles to achieving this goal. The determination of bundle orientations would have to be done quickly and automatically for realistic clinical implementation, perhaps by applying artificial intelligence approaches to derive the map. The image used to perform that task would have to cover the entire 24-2 visual field with adequate visibility. The image would also have to be acquired while the subject was seated at the perimeter, because moving to a different instrument would alter the exact position of the subject on the chin rest and hence induce torsional eye movements. If these obstacles could be overcome, it seems likely (although not certain) that it would yield further improvement in the performance of the moving stimulus relative to static stimuli; hence, again our results are conservative and the benefits of moving stimuli may be being underestimated.
A remaining question is whether the moving stimulus impacts the ability to discriminate between eyes with healthy versus damaged visual fields. In this study, all eyes had a clinical diagnosis of either glaucoma or glaucoma suspect. Testing in healthy eyes is underway to answer that question. It should be noted, however, that the usefulness of the moving stimulus technique does not depend on the results of those experiments. If defect detectability with the moving stimulus is equal to, or better than, with the static stimulus, then it would be reasonable to use a stimulus whose magnitude of movement is constant, as in the current study. If defect detectability is decreased using the moving stimulus, then the distance travelled by the stimulus could simply be scaled linearly with contrast. At near-normal sensitivities, there would be near-zero stimulus motion, ensuring that defect detectability would be identical to that achieved using static stimuli; then, the amount of motion would increase as contrast increased, providing the benefits of extended dynamic range and decreased variability at damaged locations.
The speed and distance travelled of the moving stimulus used in this study was scaled by eccentricity, as seen in
Figure 1. Some form of scaling is necessary; the amount of motion needed to increase sensitivity at central locations would be imperceptible at more peripheral locations. However, the optimal scaling to use is unclear. The stimulus used here was scaled based on the cortical magnification factor,
27 yet different formulae for that factor have been reported.
28,44 Further, it is not clear whether it would be better to scale the stimulus to obtain equal average sensitivities across the visual field for healthy observers, or to obtain equal lower limits of the normative range across the visual field, which may not give the same formula.
45,46 The magnitude of the difference between such formulae may be too small for its effect to be detectable with small scale experiments, so it is likely that a choice would have to be made a priori before any clinical implementation of the technique. It is not clear whether the greater distance travelled peripherally makes those locations more susceptible to variability caused by fixation instability, particularly when using a 500-ms stimulus, or whether the decreased axon bundle density peripherally makes those locations sufficiently robust to fixational movements. Adjustments could also be made to optimize the efficiency of the testing, for example, using spatial filtering to allow all 52 locations in the 24-2 visual field to be tested instead of the subset of 34 locations tested here; however, these should not alter the direct head-to-head comparisons between stimulus types performed in this study. A final caveat is that subjects in this study had a clinical diagnosis of glaucoma or glaucoma suspect, and so the usefulness of the technique in patients with other causes of vision loss is not yet known.
We have previously shown that the effective dynamic range of perimetry can be extended by using a larger stimulus. The same limit of 15 to 19 dB applied for both size III and size V stimuli, but the higher sensitivities with the larger stimulus meant that this limit was not reached as soon.
12 This approach could be extended, using ever larger stimuli to probe locations with increasing damage. In early damage, several groups have reported that size modulation perimetry shows promise, because it may have a better signal-to-noise ratio than conventional perimetry (where size remains constant but contrast is modulated), in particular for detection of defects.
47–49 This finding is particularly true if stimuli are configured to remain smaller than Ricco's area of complete spatial summation, which expands in glaucoma.
49 However, RGC receptive fields exhibit a center-surround organization.
50,51 The neural response of an RGC increases if the center of its receptive field is stimulated, but this response is decreased if the surround is also stimulated.
21,52 This finding implies that the largest response to a perimetric stimulus comes from RGCs whose receptive field is near the edge of the stimulus, rather than those near the middle of the stimulus where both center and surround of the receptive field are stimulated. Hence, near the detection threshold it is primarily these RGCs near the stimulus edge that determine detectability. If the stimulus size is increased, it is no longer the same RGCs that are located near the edge of the stimulus. The extent of this problem with size modulation remains to be seen, and it is possible that it may be only a relatively minor caveat, especially when seeking only to distinguish defects from areas of normal sensitivity. However, the lack of location consistency could severely impair the ability to monitor glaucomatous progression. Size modulation perimetry is implicitly assuming that RGC loss is homogeneous across the extent of the stimulus (a circle several degrees across), whereas moving stimulus perimetry only relies on the much weaker assumption that RGC loss is homogeneous within the same axon bundle, as supported by advanced imaging studies.
24,25 Notably, if it is found that these caveats with size modulation perimetry are relatively minor, it would be perfectly possible to combine the two approaches, whereby a stimulus both enlarges and moves, to obtain the benefits of both.
In summary, we found that using a moving stimulus instead of a static stimulus, in an otherwise identical seen/not seen perimetric task similar to those used clinically, both extended the dynamic range into locations with more advanced disease and decreased test–retest variability while within the dynamic range. The test could be implemented on current instruments, easing translatability of the findings and making any transition easier for patients. Subjects mostly reported preferring the moving stimulus test, suggesting potential benefits for both patient satisfaction and the reliability of the results. Given the known high variability of current perimetry especially in damaged areas, and the lack of other test modalities for monitoring disease progression in regions of advanced loss, we suggest that use of a moving stimulus may improve the diagnostic usefulness of functional testing for glaucoma.