The 30 frames per second videos were converted to frames (stills) and the interpalpebral fissure was manually measured in every frame using ImageJ software (available at
https://imagej.nih.gov/ij). Interpalpebral fissure, was defined as the greatest distance between the upper and lower eyelid margins at the base of the eye lashes as illustrated in
Figure 2. The measurements were calibrated using the average horizontal visible iris diameter of 11.67 mm in the adult population, which is very consistent across gender and race with sample standard deviations of 0.32 mm
31 to 0.42 mm.
32 In a prior work, we found horizontal visible iris diameter to be at least as reliable as using a calibration marker on the forehead.
33 Factors for analyses included spectacle-magnet angular position, eyelid-magnet type, frame type, and blink type (spontaneous or volitional).
After frame-by-frame measurements, we plotted interpalpebral fissure against time to construct the blink pattern (trace) from each video recording, an example of which is shown in
Figure 3. We separated the spontaneous blink and volitional blink periods, based on whether the participant was asked to perform a volitional blink or was allowed to blink naturally (spontaneous blink). Despite variation in the blink length between individuals and tasks, spontaneous blinks usually last a fraction of a second and may not normally be complete (full eye closure).
34,35 Typically, volitional blinks lasted more than a second with a square-wave pattern (
Fig. 3). To distinguish blink periods from eye-open periods, we employed an algorithm based on the first and second derivatives of the interpalpebral fissure trace to detect sharp falls (start of each blink cycle) and rises (end of each blink cycle) in interpalpebral fissure.
35 For volitional blinks, we considered the interpalpebral fissure points between fortieth and sixtieth percentile of the data distribution within the blink period (magenta markers in
Fig. 3). Limiting the included data to that within the middle of the within-blink period ensured that data points within the fast closing and opening phases of the blink (the sloped regions in
Fig. 3) were not included. For spontaneous blinks, the three smallest interpalpebral fissure heights were used to represent the blinking interpalpebral fissure for that blink period (red circles in
Fig. 3). This was selected over a single minimum point to reduce potential measurement biases and errors. Given the frame rate and blink duration, three points was the highest sample around the minimum that was consistently available for spontaneous blinks. Periods of resting open were defined using the distribution of all interpalpebral fissure points in the between-blink periods, with those that were within the interquartile region of 40th and 60th percentile being defined as being during resting opening (green markers in
Fig. 3). This definition ensured that the eye-open points did not include the sloped regions (i.e., falls and rises) of blinks. A representative sample of interpalpebral fissure traces were manually reviewed and selected to have both typical blinks that were easily detected as well as those that were less obvious. Multiple criteria were tested and results were manually compared to the ground truth representative sample. Through this process an algorithm that produced full accuracy of blink detection in the representative sample was produced.