January 2024
Volume 13, Issue 1
Open Access
Neuro-ophthalmology  |   January 2024
Cone-Driven, Geniculocortical Responses in Canine Models of Outer Retinal Disease
Author Affiliations & Notes
  • Huseyin O. Taskin
    Department of Neurology, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
  • Jacqueline Wivel
    Division of Experimental Retinal Therapies, Department of Clinical Sciences & Advanced Medicine, School of Veterinary Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
  • Gustavo D. Aguirre
    Division of Experimental Retinal Therapies, Department of Clinical Sciences & Advanced Medicine, School of Veterinary Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
  • William A. Beltran
    Division of Experimental Retinal Therapies, Department of Clinical Sciences & Advanced Medicine, School of Veterinary Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
  • Geoffrey K. Aguirre
    Department of Neurology, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
  • Correspondence: Geoffrey K. Aguirre, Department of Neurology, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, 3400 Spruce Street, Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA. e-mail: aguirreg@upenn.edu 
Translational Vision Science & Technology January 2024, Vol.13, 18. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/tvst.13.1.18
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Huseyin O. Taskin, Jacqueline Wivel, Gustavo D. Aguirre, William A. Beltran, Geoffrey K. Aguirre; Cone-Driven, Geniculocortical Responses in Canine Models of Outer Retinal Disease. Trans. Vis. Sci. Tech. 2024;13(1):18. https://doi.org/10.1167/tvst.13.1.18.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Purpose: Canine models of inherited retinal degeneration are used for proof of concept of emerging gene and cell-based therapies that aim to produce functional restoration of cone-mediated vision. We examined functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) measures of the postretinal response to cone-directed stimulation in wild-type (WT) dogs, and in three different retinal disease models.

Methods: Temporal spectral modulation of a uniform field of light around a photopic background was used to target the canine L/M (hereafter “L”) and S cones and rods. Stimuli were designed to separately target the postreceptoral luminance (L+S) and chrominance (L–S) pathways, the rods, and all photoreceptors jointly (light flux). These stimuli were presented to WT, and mutant PDE6B-RCD1, RPGR-XLPRA2, and NPHP5-CRD2 dogs during pupillometry and functional MRI (fMRI).

Results: Pupil responses in WT dogs to light flux, L+S, and rod-directed stimuli were consistent with responses being driven by cone signals alone. For WT animals, both luminance and chromatic (L–S) stimuli evoked fMRI responses in the lateral geniculate nucleus or visual cortex; RCD1 animals with predominant rod loss had similar responses. Responses to cone-directed stimulation were reduced in XLPRA2 and absent in CRD2. NPHP5 gene augmentation restored the cortical response to luminance stimulation in a CRD2 animal.

Conclusions: Cone-directed stimulation during fMRI can be used to measure the integrity of luminance and chrominance responses in the dog visual system. The NPHP5-CRD2 model is appealing for studies of recovered cone function.

Translational Relevance: fMRI assessment of cone-driven cortical response provides a tool to translate cell/gene therapies for vision restoration.

Introduction
The dog is an important model for studies of retinal cone disease and its treatment, owing to the availability of diverse, naturally occurring genetic disorders and to the presence of a fovea-like, cone-rich zone in the canine area centralis.1 The ultimate goal of treatment—whether by somatic cell gene therapy25 or transplantation of cone precursors6,7—is restoration of function, with vision supported by the cones under daylight conditions of particular importance. Measurement of visual behavior in post-treatment animals, however, requires training that can be difficult to start before successful therapy. Further, measuring the effect of treatment is complicated in some models by the presence of residual cone function at baseline. 
Noninvasive measurements of physiologic and neural response have been used as a proxy for visual function. Flash electroretinography (ERG) provides a measure that principally reflects responses of the photoreceptors and middle retina, integrated across eccentricity. Similarly, pupil constriction to light probes spatially integrated retinal responses and the brainstem reflex circuit. Although methodological refinements can provide spatial maps of retinal function and target cell classes, it remains the case that ERG and pupillometry are unable to confirm that restored retinal function drives signals in the cortical visual pathway. This point is of particular importance for studies of cell therapy, because the ability of transplanted photoreceptor precursor cells to recreate functional connections with the inner retina is still to be determined.6 
Visual evoked potentials measure the cortical response to visual stimulation and have been used as a measure of treatment response in canine retinal gene therapy.8 Visual evoked potentials are challenging to measure in the dog, however, owing to the small signal, risk of contamination from ERG responses, and the sensitivity of the measurement to anesthesia.9 Cortical function may also be measured in the dog using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI).10 We have previously shown that retinal gene therapy in RPE65-LCA is associated with a restoration of fMRI responses from the canine visual cortex.11 In this prior study, visual stimulation was a high photopic, flashing screen that alternated with periods of darkness. Consequently, both rod and cone responses could have contributed to the measured cortical activity. 
In the current study, we measured with fMRI cortical responses to cone-directed stimulation. In dichromatic mammals like dogs, signals from the cones contribute to two postreceptoral pathways that encode luminance (overall brightness) and chrominance (blue–yellow) (Fig. 1a). One goal of the current study was to determine whether separate responses from the luminance and chromatic pathways are measurable in dogs with no retinal disease. We also collected data from animals with each of three different inherited retinal diseases that differ in the extent of rod or cone involvement. Our results demonstrate that fMRI provides a reliable, within-animal assessment of the presence of postretinal cone signals in the dog, and that a particular disease model (NPHP5-CRD2) is especially promising for the study of recovered cone function. 
Figure 1.
 
(a) Signals from the L/M (hereafter “L”) and S cones are combined and contrasted to create two postretinal channels that signal luminance and chrominance. (b) Spectral sensitivities of the canine photoreceptors. (c) Stimulus spectra used to target particular photoreceptor combinations. Each stimulus modulated between a stimulating (red) and suppressing (black) spectrum around a common background (dashed gray). The nominal contrast on the targeted photoreceptors is given in brackets. Light flux produces equal contrast on all photoreceptors. (d) A spatially uniform field of light was presented to one eye. The field modulated between a stimulating and a suppressing spectrum, following a sinusoidal temporal waveform. In measurements of pupil response the modulation frequency was 1/6 Hz, and the consensual pupil response to the stimulus was recorded from the unstimulated eye using an infrared camera. (e) In fMRI measurements, a higher frequency flicker was presented in 12-second blocks, interleaved with presentations of the static, photopic background spectrum.
Figure 1.
 
(a) Signals from the L/M (hereafter “L”) and S cones are combined and contrasted to create two postretinal channels that signal luminance and chrominance. (b) Spectral sensitivities of the canine photoreceptors. (c) Stimulus spectra used to target particular photoreceptor combinations. Each stimulus modulated between a stimulating (red) and suppressing (black) spectrum around a common background (dashed gray). The nominal contrast on the targeted photoreceptors is given in brackets. Light flux produces equal contrast on all photoreceptors. (d) A spatially uniform field of light was presented to one eye. The field modulated between a stimulating and a suppressing spectrum, following a sinusoidal temporal waveform. In measurements of pupil response the modulation frequency was 1/6 Hz, and the consensual pupil response to the stimulus was recorded from the unstimulated eye using an infrared camera. (e) In fMRI measurements, a higher frequency flicker was presented in 12-second blocks, interleaved with presentations of the static, photopic background spectrum.
Methods
Subjects
The animals evaluated were part of a canine research colony maintained at the University of Pennsylvania, Retinal Diseases Studies Facility. All procedures were carried out in strict accordance with the ARVO Statement for the Use of Animals in Ophthalmic and Vision Research and approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of Pennsylvania (IACUC no. 803254). The study examined wild-type (WT) dogs, and dogs affected with late-stage inherited retinal degeneration caused by mutations in PDE6B (RCD1 model),12 RPGR (XLPRA2 model),13 and NPHP5/IQCB1 (CRD2 model).14 None of the dogs in this study were neutered. A description of the studied animals is provided in Table, and the known impairments in retinal and visual function in these disease models are described in Supplementary Table S1. To summarize, RCD1 has severe rod loss with relatively preserved cone function, whereas XLPRA2 and CRD2 both have diminished cone function, with this loss being nearly complete in CRD2. 
Table.
 
Summary of the Studied Animals
Table.
 
Summary of the Studied Animals
The primary study collected fMRI data from three RCD1, five XLPRA2, three CRD2, and three WT animals; data from a fourth RCD1 animal (ID:2353) were collected but were unusable owing to a temporal noise source of unclear cause. A subset of these animals was also studied with pupillometry: three RCD1, three XLPRA2, and two WT (one WT animal could not be studied owing to the inability to achieve a suitable level of anesthesia). 
An additional CRD2 animal was studied with fMRI 32 weeks after retinal gene therapy to one eye.5 This same animal had undergone ERG and visual behavior assessment in an obstacle avoidance course at 24 weeks after treatment using previously described methods.15 Finally, an additional WT animal was studied with a different, localizer fMRI protocol for the purposes of defining the anatomical location of the lateral geniculate and visual cortex. 
Anesthesia and Animal Preparation
The animals were given subcutaneous acepromazine (0.25 mg/kg) and atropine (0.02 mg/kg) before transportation to the experimental site. The time between the administration of this drug and induction varied, but was at least 1.5 hours. Anti-nausea medication (maropitant citrate, Cerenia, Zoetis) was given 20 minutes before induction. Induction was performed with ketamine (1–10 mg/kg IV) and midazolam (0.2–0.5 mg/kg IV). A loading dose of dexmedetomidine (2–5 µg/kg) was used, and anesthesia was maintained during the experiment with ketamine (5–15 mg/kg/h IV), midazolam (0.2–0.6 mg/kg/h IV), and dexmedetomidine (1–3 µg/kg/h IV). The fMRI studies were conducted under pharmacological paralysis; vecuronium was given as a loading dose (0.1 mg/kg) and then a maintenance dose (0.1–0.2 mg/kg/h) during scanning. Paralysis was not used for the pupillometry studies. 
During all data collection, animals were intubated and mechanically ventilated with 100% O2. Respiratory rates that were multiples of five breaths per minute were avoided to avoid the introduction of cardiopulmonary signals at harmonics of the fMRI stimulus frequency. Pulse oximetry, end-tidal CO2, and core body temperature were monitored throughout data collection. 
For the fMRI studies, both eyes were pharmacologically dilated with topical atropine, tropicamide, and phenylephrine once when the dogs arrived at the experimental site and again 20 to 30 minutes before induction; pupil dilation was not performed for the pupillometry studies because the goal was to measure the consensual pupil response. The eyelids were held open during fMRI studies with sprung plastic specula and during pupillometry with lid sutures. The eyes were lubricated at frequent intervals throughout the study with artificial tears. Earplugs and foam padding over the ears were provided during fMRI scanning. Room lights were turned off. 
Spectral Modulations
Light stimuli were produced by a digital light synthesis engine (OneLight Spectra), conveyed via a fiber optic cable, and then presented to the animal within a custom-made, MRI-compatible, circular eyepiece with an approximately 26° uniform visual field.16 Intermittent calibration of the device and regular measurements of the spectral properties of the stimuli was performed with a spectroradiometer (PhotoResearch PR-670). During data collection, the eyepiece was held with an articulated plastic arm and positioned approximately 5 mm from the corneal surface of the stimulated eye. The fellow eye was uncovered during data collection. During the pupillometry sessions, an infrared video camera (640 × 480 pixel resolution at 60 Hz interlaced; LiveTrack, Cambridge Research Systems, Rochester, Kent, UK) was used to record the pupil response from the nonstimulated eye. 
The stimuli for both experiments targeted particular photoreceptor classes using the method of silent substitution.17,18 Stimuli were designed to stimulate or silence the L cone (also termed the L/M or ML cone in dichromatic animals), S cone, rod, and melanopsin photopigments (Fig. 1b). The spectral sensitivity of these photoreceptor classes was modeled using the Govardovskii nomogram,19 with a lambda max of 555, 429, 506, and 480 nm, for the L, S, rhodopsin, and melanopsin photopigments, respectively. The spectral transmittance of the canine crystalline lens was included in the calculation to account for prereceptoral filtering.20 A nonlinear search across device settings was used to construct spectral modulations that had the desired property of silencing some photoreceptors while maximizing contrast on targeted photoreceptors (Fig. 1c). The modulations were presented around a common, half-on spectral background. Neutral density filters were placed in the light path to bring the background into the desired luminance range. The mean ± standard deviation human luminance of the stimulus across experiments was 438 ± 83 cd/m2 for pupillometry, and 305 ± 92 cd/m2 for MRI, which corresponds with a corneal irradiance of 0.29 ± 0.05 and 0.20 ± 0.06 watts/m2 respectively. Spectroradiometric measurements of the stimuli were made immediately before and after each data collection session. Supplementary Table S2 provides the calculated contrast on the targeted and nominally silenced photoreceptors. 
Pupillometry Stimulus and Data Analysis
In each of many acquisitions, a bipolar modulation of the spectral content of the stimulus was presented to one eye, and the consensual pupil response was recorded from the fellow eye under closed-loop conditions. The modulation followed a slow (1/6 Hz) sinusoidal temporal profile of transition from one arm of the spectral pair to the other, passing through the background spectrum (Fig. 1d). Each acquisition was 360 seconds in duration. Three acquisitions were obtained in order using the L+S, Rod + Mel, and Light Flux modulations. The stimulated and recorded eyes were then switched, and this set of acquisitions was repeated. A total of 12 acquisitions were made in a measurement session for a given animal. An external transistor–transistor logic pulse was used to initiate both the stimulus and the video recording, establishing a common temporal reference. 
The resulting infra-red (IR) videos were processed using previously described open-source software.21,22 The primary operation of the analysis was to fit an ellipse to the contrast-defined border between the pupil and iris for each frame of the video. Imperfections in the border segmentation (owing to, for example, the presence of eyelashes or the first Purkinje reflex of the active IR source) were removed by iteratively constraining the parameters of the ellipse and removing poorly fit border points. In some cases, translational motion correction was applied to the images to correct head motion during the recording interval. Acquisitions in which more than 50% of video frames contained poor pupil fits were excluded from the analysis; four acquisitions were so excluded, out of the total of 96 acquisitions collected. 
The ellipse area over time was expressed as percentage change and the time series data were fit by linear regression with a sine and cosine at 1/6 Hz. The resulting amplitude and phase of the fit were obtained. The standard error of the mean of these fits was estimated by repeating the fit across an exhaustive bootstrap resampling of all 35 available combinations of the 4 measurements sampled with replacement and taking the standard deviation of the set of values. 
MRI Stimulus and Image Preprocessing
In each of many fMRI acquisitions, a rapidly flickering spectral modulation was presented to one eye of the animal. The flicker was presented in 12-second blocks, alternating with 12 seconds of the steady, photopic background spectrum. The flicker within each stimulation block was subject to a 1.5-second half-cosine temporal window at onset and offset (Fig. 1e). Each acquisition was 432 seconds in duration. Three acquisitions were obtained, in order, using the L+S, L–S, and light flux modulations. The flicker frequency of these stimuli was 32 Hz, 4 Hz, and 16 Hz, respectively, selected based on the temporal sensitivity of canine vision.23,24 The set of three acquisitions was collected for one eye, then the stimulus was switched to the other eye, and the acquisition set was repeated. A total of 18 acquisitions were acquired in a given scanning session. 
MRIs were performed on a 3T Trio Siemens scanner with a 15-channel knee coil. Two MPRAGE images were collected at the beginning of each scan with the *tfl3d1 sequence, 0.729 × 0.7 × 0.729 voxel size, with a repetition time (TR) of 1700 ms, an echo time of 4.87ms, and a flip angle of 9°. The blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) images were collected with the epfid2d1_64 sequence, 2 × 2 × 3 voxel size, a TR of 3000 ms, an echo time of 30 ms, and a flip angle of 90°. Immediately after acquiring the MPRAGE images, two single-TR scout images with the same parameters as the BOLD images were obtained with alternating anteroposterior and posteroanterior phase encoding directions, and these were used for susceptibility distortion correction. The main BOLD sequences were also collected with alternating phase encoding directions. 
For each subject, the two structural images were bias-corrected with N4 bias correction25 and spatially aligned with linear registration. The registered images were averaged. An initial approximate brain mask was created on the resulting image with the FSL Brain Extraction Tool.26 This mask was used as an input to the altAntsBrainExtraction algorithm to perform skull stripping (https://github.com/cookpa/altAntsBrainExtraction). The skull-stripped image was then warped to a canine template27 with nonlinear diffeomorphic (SyN) registration.28 
A susceptibility distortion field map was calculated from the anteroposterior and posteroanterior scout image pairs using FSL top-up.29 This field map was used to perform distortion correction on the scout and fMRI time-series images. The resulting time series images were motion corrected with rigid body registration using the middle TR volume as the target,30 and the mean volumetric image across time was obtained from the motion-corrected data. This mean volume was linearly registered to one of the top-up corrected scout images, and then warped to the canine template with a single interpolation using the warp fields that were calculated previously between the structural and template coordinates. 
Occasional transient, high-amplitude spikes were observed in the raw time series data in some sessions (attributed to electrical arcing at the coil plug sockets). We created an algorithm to detect these spikes. First, the linear trend was removed from the raw functional images, and then outlier detection was performed for each voxel across time by calculating the median absolute deviation. A voxel was considered an outlier if it was at least 6 median absolute deviations above the median. This detection resulted in a vector containing the number of noisy voxels for each TR in an image. With another outlier detection performed on this vector, we identified time points whose number of noisy voxels was at least 25 median absolute deviations above the median value and created confound regressors for these time points. 
Although the animal was paralyzed during scanning, brain motion was still present owing to cardiopulmonary activity. For each acquisition, 24 motion regressors were obtained from the least-squares motion correction operation (original parameters, temporal derivatives, and squares of both). A principal components analysis was then performed upon this set of regressors and a reduced set of vectors that explained 95% of the variance was obtained. Additional vectors for each image spike were added, and the variance attributable to the resulting confound matrix was regressed from each voxel. 
Because we used a uniform field, monocular stimulus, and given that the dog has approximately 60° of binocular vision,31 we might expect that postchiasmal neural responses would be larger in the eye contralateral to the stimulus as compared with ipsilateral. Consistent with this hypothesis, we found a reliable difference in cortical response between the two hemispheres in WT animals in response to the light flux stimulus (Supplementary Fig. S1). Although the overall magnitude of response may differ between the hemispheres, we had no a priori reason to think that this difference would interact with the effect of photoreceptor direction or disease model. Therefore, we combined responses from the left and right brain hemispheres by mirror reversing the preprocessed time series data around the sagittal plane, performing a linear registration between the original and flipped images, and then averaging them together. 
fMRI Model Fitting
The fMRI time-series data from each imaging session was fitted using an open source, nonlinear model fitting routine (https://github.com/gkaguirrelab/forwardModel). For each session, the set of six acquisitions that were obtained for a particular stimulus type were concatenated, combining across left and right eye stimulation. The model consisted of a square-wave representation of the 12-second on and off blocks of the stimulus, with the amplitude of this response under the control of a different parameter for each of the six acquisitions. The square wave representation was then convolved with a model of the hemodynamic response function, itself defined by three parameters that describe a weighted combination of a three-component, hemodynamic basis set.32 A nonlinear search was performed to identify model parameters that minimized the L2 norm of this model fit to the observed data in each voxel. The mean amplitude of response (beta) was obtained and averaged for each disease model and stimulus modulation. An additional analysis was performed for data averaged across voxels within predefined ROIs (defined below) and results were plotted for each subject separately. 
Region of Interest Definition
We defined visually responsive regions of interest using localizer data collected from a separate WT animal. The stimulation used for this localizer measurement was the 12-second, blocked 16 Hz light flux flicker around a photopic (approximately 400 cd/m2) background, alternating with 12-second periods of darkness (approximately 1 cd/m2). A total of 44 acquisitions were obtained in three separate sessions. Anesthesia, MRI parameters, data preprocessing, and statistical analysis were performed as described elsewhere in this article and a map of the R2 of the model fit to the across-acquisition, average time-series was obtained. The resulting map was thresholded and binarized, yielding two discrete regions corresponding with the visual cortex (143 voxels in size), and the lateral geniculate nucleus (27 voxels in size). These two regions were then used to interrogate the data collected from other studied animals. 
Results
Pupil Responses to Cone-directed Stimuli Show Minimal Signs of Rod Intrusion
We measured consensual pupil responses to slow (1/6 Hz) sinusoidal modulations of the spectral content of a field of light presented to one eye. The first of these modulations, “light flux,” produces high-contrast, joint stimulation of the cones, rods, and melanopsin, although the photopic stimulus background might be expected to saturate rod responses, and the stimulus frequency is fast relative to melanopsin kinetics.16 A clear pupillary response at the stimulation frequency was seen in the across-animal, average response for each of the three studied groups (Fig. 2, top). 
Figure 2.
 
Mean (across animals) pupil response to three different stimulus conditions: light flux (which drives all photoreceptor classes), L+S (cone luminance, silencing the rods and melanopsin), and Rod+Mel (silencing the cones). Measurements were made for three groups of animals (WT, RCD1, and XLPRA2). The measured pupil response is in gray, and the fit of a sinusoidal response model is in red, with the thin lines indicating the standard error of the mean across all acquisitions from all animals in a group.
Figure 2.
 
Mean (across animals) pupil response to three different stimulus conditions: light flux (which drives all photoreceptor classes), L+S (cone luminance, silencing the rods and melanopsin), and Rod+Mel (silencing the cones). Measurements were made for three groups of animals (WT, RCD1, and XLPRA2). The measured pupil response is in gray, and the fit of a sinusoidal response model is in red, with the thin lines indicating the standard error of the mean across all acquisitions from all animals in a group.
Next, we measured the pupil response to a modulation designed to produce isolated L and S cone stimulation. Because of the need to tailor the spectral modulation to nominally silence the rods and melanopsin, the contrast of this stimulus upon the cones was about a third of that produced by light flux (35% vs 95%). This L+S stimulus also evoked a pupillary response in all three disease models, although relatively reduced in XLPRA2 (Fig. 2, middle). As would be predicted by the decrease in stimulus contrast, the amplitude of the pupil response to the L+S stimulus was roughly 30% of that produced by the light flux stimulus (ratio of L+S response amplitude to light flux response amplitude: 0.34 in WT; 0.28 in RCD1; 0.25 in XLPRA2). 
Finally, we measured the response to a stimulus that produced substantial (50%) contrast on the rods and melanopsin while silencing the cones. There was effectively no response in the WT and XLPRA2 groups, and only a small response was observed in RCD1. 
Figure 3 presents the measured response for each animal and stimulus condition in a polar plot representation. While there was considerable variation between individual animals in the amplitude of response, phase was consistent across animals for the light flux and L+S stimulus conditions. No difference was found in phase between the light flux and L+S stimuli across animals (paired t test [7 df] = 0.21; P = 0.84). This finding suggests that slower melanopsin and rod signals make a minimal contribution to the light flux response.16,33 Responses to the Rod+Mel condition were of low amplitude and variable phase. 
Figure 3.
 
Polar representation of pupil responses to the three stimulus conditions for each studied animal. Distance from the origin indicates the amplitude of pupil change, and angular position indicates the phase delay. The standard error of the mean of the response measured for each animal (across acquisitions) is indicated by the error bars.
Figure 3.
 
Polar representation of pupil responses to the three stimulus conditions for each studied animal. Distance from the origin indicates the amplitude of pupil change, and angular position indicates the phase delay. The standard error of the mean of the response measured for each animal (across acquisitions) is indicated by the error bars.
Brain Response to Cone-directed Stimulation Differs Across Disease Models
We created a flat-map representation of the canine posterior cortex (Fig. 4) with the location of visual area borders defined by homology with the cat.34 
Figure 4.
 
Flattened representation of the canine visual cortex. The marginal, supra-splenial, and splenial sulci are indicated. The dots show the expected position of the upper and lower vertical meridians (UVM, LVM) and the dashes show the horizontal meridian (HM). The foveal confluence is indicated with a star.
Figure 4.
 
Flattened representation of the canine visual cortex. The marginal, supra-splenial, and splenial sulci are indicated. The dots show the expected position of the upper and lower vertical meridians (UVM, LVM) and the dashes show the horizontal meridian (HM). The foveal confluence is indicated with a star.
Figure 5 presents the mean fMRI response for the stimuli and studied populations. The response from the visual cortex and the lateral geniculate nucleus is shown; minimal responses were found at other anatomical sites (see Supplementary Fig. S2). 
Figure 5.
 
fMRI responses to cone-directed stimulation. The average, across animal (and hemisphere) response (percent signal change), is shown for each of three stimuli and each of four studied populations. Cortical responses are shown on a flat map, and subcortical response on an axial slice that passes through the LGN. Data from the two hemispheres were combined, resulting in symmetric maps. The location of the horizontal (dashes) and vertical (dots) meridians are indicated. Different map thresholds were used for the cortical surface and subcortical maps (see color bar). Responses in the cortex and thalamus are of opposite sign, perhaps owing to the effects of anesthesia.
Figure 5.
 
fMRI responses to cone-directed stimulation. The average, across animal (and hemisphere) response (percent signal change), is shown for each of three stimuli and each of four studied populations. Cortical responses are shown on a flat map, and subcortical response on an axial slice that passes through the LGN. Data from the two hemispheres were combined, resulting in symmetric maps. The location of the horizontal (dashes) and vertical (dots) meridians are indicated. Different map thresholds were used for the cortical surface and subcortical maps (see color bar). Responses in the cortex and thalamus are of opposite sign, perhaps owing to the effects of anesthesia.
In the cortex, luminance directed stimuli (light flux and L+S) evoked fMRI responses in the vicinity of the supra-splenial sulcus, straddling the horizontal meridian and close to the vertical meridian border. This response was seen in both the WT and the RCD1 animals, both of which have intact (WT) and retained (RCD1) cone function at the studied ages. The two disease models with cone impairment (XLPRA2 and CRD2) did not have responses at this threshold. A corresponding pattern of responses in the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) was also seen. 
The isoluminant L–S stimulus did not produce a cortical response in the WT or disease model animals. There was, however, a measurable LGN response to this stimulus in the WT animal, demonstrating a response within the canine geniculocortical pathway to chromatic stimulation. 
We quantified these responses within cortical and LGN regions-of-interest. Figure 6 presents the fMRI response for each studied animal, grouped by stimulus and disease model. 
Figure 6.
 
fMRI responses (% signal change) within the visual cortex (left) and LGN (right) for each of the three stimulus types (rows), organized by disease model. Error bars are the standard error of the mean across fMRI acquisitions obtained for an individual animal. Note the different y-axis direction and range for the cortex and LGN plots.
Figure 6.
 
fMRI responses (% signal change) within the visual cortex (left) and LGN (right) for each of the three stimulus types (rows), organized by disease model. Error bars are the standard error of the mean across fMRI acquisitions obtained for an individual animal. Note the different y-axis direction and range for the cortex and LGN plots.
The light flux and L+S stimuli evoke some small responses in the XLPRA2 animals, despite their extensive cone loss. In comparison, there is consistently no response to the cone-directed stimulation in the CRD2 population. 
Gene Therapy Restores Cone-selective Functional Responses in the Visual Cortex
We studied one CRD2 animal approximately 32 weeks after retinal gene therapy to the left eye. This animal received a subretinal injection at 14 weeks of age (midstage disease) of an adeno-associate virus 2/5 vector carrying a single-stranded construct composed of the human NPHP5 cDNA with a woodchuck hepatitis virus post-transcriptional regulatory element that was under control of the photoreceptor-specific GRK1 promoter. Figure 7 shows the cortical response in this animal to light flux stimulation around a photopic background delivered to the left eye, and to the untreated right eye. A clear difference in evoked cortical response is seen, confirming that we are able to measure recovered cone-specific function in this disease model (mean response within the cortical region of interest ± standard error of the mean across acquisitions: left eye, 0.229% ± 0.094%; right eye, 0.022% ± 0.086%). The L+S and L–S stimuli did not evoke a reliable response in the cortex from either eye and there was no measurable response in the LGN to any of the modulation types. Full-field ERG and testing of visually guided behavior 24 weeks after subretinal injection of the adeno-associated virus 2/5-NPHP5 vector had shown in the left (treated) eye rescue of both rod- and cone-mediated function (Supplementary Figs. S3 and S4). 
Figure 7.
 
fMRI responses (percent signal change) to light flux stimulation within the visual cortex of a CRD2 animal following gene therapy delivered to the left eye.
Figure 7.
 
fMRI responses (percent signal change) to light flux stimulation within the visual cortex of a CRD2 animal following gene therapy delivered to the left eye.
Discussion
Manipulation of the spectral content of stimuli has been used previously in the dog to probe photoreceptor contributions in both ERG and pupil responses.35,36 Our work extends these techniques to fMRI measurements in normal and retinal disease models (as has been pursued in the human).37,38 In WT dogs, we find reliable responses in the geniculocortical pathway to both luminance and chromatic stimuli. Measurements from inherited retinal disease models support both the selectivity of our stimuli for eliciting cone-driven responses and help to identify a promising disease model for the study of cone function restoration. 
The three disease models we studied differ in the degree of impairment of rod and cone function. In dogs with PDE6B-RCD1, approximately 70% of rods are lost in the outer nuclear layer by 3 months of age, but cones are relatively spared.39,40 By the age of 6 to 8 months (at which we made the bulk of our measurements), further rod loss would be expected. RPGR-XLPRA2 affects both cones and rods and results in the loss of one-third of these photoreceptors by 3 to 4 months of age,4,41 with further loss of both classes expected by the age of 6 to 18 months we studied. CRD2 owing to NPHP5 mutation features severe, early cone dysfunction followed by a secondary rod loss.5,42 At the ages studied here (5–7 months) almost complete loss of rod and cone function is found. 
Normative Canine Visual Cortex Responses
The retinotopic organization of the visual cortex has not been studied in the dog, although electrophysiological34 and fMRI measurements44 have been made in the cat. The location of maximal cortical response in our data corresponds with the primary visual cortex, nearly centered on the foveal representation. Although electrophysiological data from the cat locate the fovea on the marginal gyrus, our data (and the cat fMRI study)44 find the peak response in the adjacent suprasplenial sulcus. The elliptical extent of the cortical response we observe corresponds to approximately 10° radial eccentricity,34 which is in rough agreement with the 13° radial extent of our stimulus.45 
We found reliable fMRI responses to chromatic (L–S) stimuli in the LGN of WT dogs, although there was not an accompanying cortical response. Although the S-cones are relatively sparse in the canine retina,46 dogs are known to be able to distinguish luminance and blue–yellow chrominance.47 Our study used a relatively slow, 4 Hz L–S flicker stimulus, based on the low-pass characteristic of the S-cone ERG response24 and by analogy to the temporal sensitivity of human vision for isolated S-cone modulations.48 Visual cortex responses may have been reduced by habituation to this slow modulation. Future studies of canine chromatic responses might examine more rapid flicker, and make use of a shifted spectral background to achieve higher contrast on this channel. 
Photoreceptor Targeting
We used temporal variation in the entire wavelength spectrum of our stimulus to target and nominally silence specific photoreceptor classes. The design of the stimuli, and the accuracy of photoreceptor targeting, is based on physical measurements of our display apparatus and prior measurements of the spectral properties of the dog photoreceptor opsins and lens transmittance. We might consider the consequence of errors in stimulus targeting. Perhaps of greatest concern are false positive results, in which a measured physiologic response is attributed to a particular photoreceptor signal (e.g., the cones), but is in fact owing to inadvertent stimulation of a different photoreceptor (e.g., the rods). 
We characterized the effect of measured imprecision in the spectral output of our stimulus device (Supplementary Table S2). The effect of deviations here are quite small. For example, inadvertent contrast upon the rods was less than 1% for the stimulus that attempted to produce isolated L+S stimulation. There is also inevitable deviation in the spectral sensitivity of the biological system from that assumed from tabular values. Although estimates of population variation in opsin sensitivity and lens transmittance are available for humans, we do not have this information for the dog. In the current measurements, for example, it is possible that a large deviation from assumed biological values resulted in luminance contrast in our nominally isoluminant L–S stimulus. Future studies could vary the contrast and temporal frequency of stimulation to test for the different signatures of the chromatic and luminance pathways, and thus explore the quality of photoreceptor isolation. 
We have positive evidence from our study that any inadvertent signaling from the rods was minimal, as might be expected given the photopic background light level we used.43 In the pupil, the similar phase of response to the light flux and L+S modulations suggests that only cone signals were evoked by the light flux stimulus, and there was minimal pupil response to rod-directed stimulation. Although our rod-directed stimulus also places contrast on canine melanopsin, the modulation frequency was too fast to expect a large pupil response from this mechanism.16 In the fMRI data, the similar amplitude of response to light flux stimulation in the WT and RCD1 animals, and the absence of responses in the CRD2 model, which has extensive cone degeneration at the ages studied, argues against a substantial rod contribution. 
The Effects of Anesthesia on Pupillary and Cortical Responses
We collected data under general anesthesia using 100% oxygen by ventilation. The presence and level of anesthesia can influence measurements of the pupillary light reflex.49,50 Anesthesia can also attenuate cortical responses to sensory stimulation, lowering the sensitivity of the measurement. Further, anesthesia and nonphysiological blood gasses can alter the relationships between sensory stimulation, neural response, and neurovascular coupling.51,52 These mechanisms likely explain the paradoxical (negative) BOLD response to stimulation in the LGN we find here (and in our prior study with dogs),11 and the difference in the sign of response between cortical and thalamic sites. Although the absolute amplitude of fMRI response is readily interpretable in our data, we would ideally have a full understanding of this signal transformation. 
CRD2 Is a Promising Model in Which to Study Recovery of Cone Function
Residual responses from spared photoreceptors as seen in the RCD1 and XLPRA2 dogs (Supplementary Table S1) complicates the assessment of efficacy of vision restoration treatments. For example, a small amount of intact cone function is sufficient for animals to demonstrate visual behavior and avoid obstacles.53 For both behavioral and neural measures, it is easier to detect a small therapeutic effect against a consistent background of no function than it is to measure a change in a nonzero response. 
Both XLPRA2 and CRD2 are associated with cone dysfunction and loss, but differ in severity and tempo; in our data, both show a decrease in neural response to cone-directed stimulation. We find, however, that NPHP5-CRD2 animals consistently show no measurable thalamic or cortical response to even the high-contrast light flux modulation. This finding is in agreement with electrophysiological and behavioral studies that show an absence of both rod- and cone-mediated function at similar ages (Supplementary Table S1). Against this background, the effect of treatment is readily apparent in the fMRI data from an individual animal, with a restoration of a normal extent of cortical response to cone-directed stimulation. 
Our measure of the amplitude of pupil response was highly variable across individual animals, perhaps related to differences in pupil size and thus retinal irradiance.35 Generally, the effect of treatment in the pupil response is measured for full-field, high-intensity flashes of light presented against a dark background.11,54,55 Pupil responses for modulations of cone-directed stimulation around a photopic background are inevitably smaller.35 We found in our data that fMRI measures provided more consistent results within individual animals and was more sensitive to group differences owing to retinal disease. 
Acknowledgments
Study Funding: P30-EY001583: Core Grant for Vision Research; U24-EY029890, RO1-EY006855, RO1-EY017549, Fighting Blindness Canada Vision 20/20, The Foundation Fighting Blindness. Low Vision Research Award from the Research to Prevent Blindness/Lions Clubs International Foundation and Sanford and Susan Greenberg End Blindness Outstanding Achievement Prize. 
Author Contributions: Conceptualization: Huseyin O. Taskin, Gustavo D. Aguirre, William A. Beltran, and Geoffrey K. Aguirre; Data curation: Huseyin O. Taskin and Geoffrey K. Aguirre; Formal analysis: Huseyin O. Taskin and Geoffrey K. Aguirre; Funding acquisition: Gustavo D. Aguirre, William A. Beltran, and Geoffrey K. Aguirre; Investigation: Jacqueline Wivel; Resources: Gustavo D. Aguirre and William A. Beltran; Software: Huseyin O. Taskin and Geoffrey K. Aguirre; Supervision: William A. Beltran and Geoffrey K. Aguirre; Visualization: Huseyin O. Taskin and Geoffrey K. Aguirre; Writing - original draft: Huseyin O. Taskin, William A. Beltran, and Geoffrey K. Aguirre; Writing - review & editing: Huseyin O. Taskin, Jacqueline Wivel, Gustavo D. Aguirre, William A. Beltran, and Geoffrey K. Aguirre. 
Disclosure: H.O. Taskin, None; J. Wivel, None; G.D. Aguirre, None; W.A. Beltran, None; G.K. Aguirre, None 
References
Beltran WA, Cideciyan AV, Guziewicz KE, et al. Canine retina has a primate fovea-like bouquet of cone photoreceptors which is affected by inherited macular degenerations. PLoS One. 2014; 9(3): e90390, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0090390. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Acland GM, Aguirre GD, Ray J, et al. Gene therapy restores vision in a canine model of childhood blindness. Nature Genet. 2001; 28(1): 92–95, https://doi.org/10.1038/ng0501-92. [PubMed]
Komáromy AM, Alexander JJ, Rowlan JS, et al. Gene therapy rescues cone function in congenital achromatopsia. Human Mol Genet. 2010; 19(13): 2581–2593, https://doi.org/10.1093/hmg/ddq136. [CrossRef]
Beltran WA, Cideciyan AV, Lewin AS, et al. Gene therapy rescues photoreceptor blindness in dogs and paves the way for treating human X-linked retinitis pigmentosa. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012; 109(6): 2132–2137, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1118847109. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Aguirre GD, Cideciyan AV, Dufour VL, et al. Gene therapy reforms photoreceptor structure and restores vision in NPHP5-associated Leber congenital amaurosis. Mol Ther. 2021; 29(8): 2456–2468, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ymthe.2021.03.021. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ludwig AL, Gamm DM. Outer retinal cell replacement: putting the pieces together. Transl Vis Sci Technol. 2021; 10(10): 15, https://doi.org/10.1167/tvst.10.10.15. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ripolles-Garcia A, Dolgova N, Phillips MJ, et al. Systemic immunosuppression promotes survival and integration of subretinally implanted human ESC-derived photoreceptor precursors in dogs. Stem Cell Rep. 2022; 17(8): 1824–1841, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.stemcr.2022.06.009. [CrossRef]
Nikonov S, Aravand P, Lyubarsky A, et al. Restoration of vision and retinal responses after adeno-associated virus–mediated optogenetic therapy in blind dogs. Transl Vis Sci Technol. 2022; 11(5): 24, https://doi.org/10.1167/tvst.11.5.24. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Strain GM, Jackson RM, Tedford BL. Visual evoked potentials in the clinically normal dog. J Vet Intern Med. 1990; 4(4): 222–225, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1939-1676.1990.tb00901.x. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Willis CK, Quinn RP, McDonell WM, Gati J, Partlow G, Vilis T. Functional MRI activity in the thalamus and occipital cortex of anesthetized dogs induced by monocular and binocular stimulation. Can J Vet Res. 2001; 65(3): 188–195, https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11480525. [PubMed]
Aguirre GK, Komáromy AM, Cideciyan AV, et al. Canine and human visual cortex intact and responsive despite early retinal blindness from RPE65 mutation. PLoS Med. 2007; 4(6): e230, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.0040230. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ray K, Baldwin VJ, Acland GM, Blanton SH, Aguirre GD. Cosegregation of codon 807 mutation of the canine rod cGMP phosphodiesterase beta gene and rcd1. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1994; 35(13): 4291–4299, https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/8002249/. [PubMed]
Zhang Q, Acland GM, Wu WX, et al. Different RPGR exon ORF15 mutations in Canids provide insights into photoreceptor cell degeneration. Human Mol Genet. 2002; 11(9): 993–1003, https://doi.org/10.1093/hmg/11.9.993. [CrossRef]
Goldstein O, Mezey JG, Schweitzer PA, et al. IQCB1 and PDE6B mutations cause similar early onset retinal degenerations in two closely related terrier dog breeds. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2013; 54(10): 7005–7019, https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.13-12915. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Beltran WA, Cideciyan AV, Iwabe S, et al. Successful arrest of photoreceptor and vision loss expands the therapeutic window of retinal gene therapy to later stages of disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2015; 112(43): E5844–E5853, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1509914112. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Spitschan M, Jain S, Brainard DH, Aguirre GK. Opponent melanopsin and S-cone signals in the human pupillary light response. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2014; 111(43): 15568–15572, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1400942111. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Donner KO, Rushton WAH. Retinal stimulation by light substitution. J Physiol. 1959; 149(2): 288–302, https://doi.org/10.1113%2Fjphysiol.1959.sp006340. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Estévez O, Spekreijse H. The “silent substitution” method in visual research. Vis Res. 1982; 22(6): 681–691, https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(82)90104-3. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Govardovskii VI, Fyhrquist N, Reuter T, Kuzmin DG, Donner K. In search of the visual pigment template. Vis Neurosci. 2000; 17(4): 509–528, https://doi.org/10.1017/s0952523800174036. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Douglas RH, Jeffery G. The spectral transmission of ocular media suggests ultraviolet sensitivity is widespread among mammals. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2014; 281(1780): 20132995, https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2013.2995. [CrossRef]
Aguirre GK . A model of the entrance pupil of the human eye. Sci Rep. 2019; 9(1): 9360, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-45827-3. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Go CC, Taskin HO, Ahmadi S-A, et al. Persistent horizontal and vertical, MR-induced nystagmus in resting state Human Connectome Project data. NeuroImage. 2022; 255: 119170, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119170. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Coile DC, Pollitz CH, Smith JC. Behavioral determination of critical flicker fusion in dogs. Physiol Behav. 1989; 45(6): 1087–1092, https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(89)90092-9. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Wise EN, Foster ML, Kremers J, Mowat FM. A modified silent substitution electroretinography protocol to separate photoreceptor subclass function in lightly sedated dogs. Vet Ophthalmol. 2021; 24(1): 103–107, https://doi.org/10.1111/vop.12847. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Tustison NJ, Avants BB, Cook PA, et al. N4ITK: improved N3 bias correction. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. 2010; 29(6): 1310–1320, https://doi.org/10.1109/TMI.2010.2046908. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Smith SM . Fast robust automated brain extraction. Human Brain Map. 2002; 17(3): 143–155, https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.10062. [CrossRef]
Datta R, Lee J, Duda J, et al. A digital atlas of the dog brain. PLoS One. 2012; 7(12): e52140, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0052140. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Avants BB, Tustison NJ, Song G, Cook PA, Klein A, Gee JC. A reproducible evaluation of ANTs similarity metric performance in brain image registration. NeuroImage. 2011; 54(3): 2033–2044, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.025. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Andersson JLR, Skare S, Ashburner J. How to correct susceptibility distortions in spin-echo echo-planar images: application to diffusion tensor imaging. NeuroImage. 2003; 20(2): 870–888, https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00336-7. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Jenkinson M, Bannister P, Brady M, Smith S. Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. NeuroImage. 2002; 17(2): 825–841, https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1132. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Miller PE, Murphy CJ. Vision in dogs. J Am Vet Med Assoc. 1995; 207(12): 1623–1634, https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7493905. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Woolrich MW, Behrens TEJ, Beckmann CF, Jenkinson M, Smith SM. Multilevel linear modelling for FMRI group analysis using Bayesian inference. NeuroImage. 2004; 21(4): 1732–1747, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.023. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kelbsch C, Strasser T, Chen Y, et al. Standards in Pupillography. Front Neurol. 2019; 10: 1–25, https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2019.00129. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Tusa RJ, Palmer LA, Rosenquist AC. The retinotopic organization of area 17 (striate cortex) in the cat. J Comp Neurol. 1978; 177(2): 213–235, https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.901770204. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Yeh C, Koehl K, Harman C, et al. Assessment of rod, cone, and intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cell contributions to the canine chromatic pupillary response. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2017; 58: 65, https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.16-19865. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Mowat FM, Wise E, Oh A, Foster ML, Kremers J. In vivo electroretinographic differentiation of rod, short-wavelength and long/medium-wavelength cone responses in dogs using silent substitution stimuli. Exp Eye Res. 2019; 185: 107673, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.exer.2019.05.013. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Gentile CP, Spitschan M, Taskin HO, Bock AS, Aguirre GK. Temporal sensitivity for achromatic and chromatic flicker across the visual cortex. BioRxiv. 2023; 2023.07.24.550403, https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.07.24.550403.
Farahbakhsh M, Anderson EJ, Maimon-Mor RO, et al. A demonstration of cone function plasticity after gene therapy in achromatopsia. Brain J Neurol. 2022; 145(11): 3803–3815, https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awac226. [CrossRef]
Aquirre G, Farber D, Lolley R, Fletcher RT, Chader GJ. Rod-cone dysplasia in Irish Setters: a defect in cyclic GMP metabolism in visual cells. Science. 1978; 201(4361): 1133–1134, https://doi.org/10.1126/science.210508. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Buyukmihci N, Aguirre G, Marshall J. Retinal degenerations in the dog II. Development of the retina in rod-cone dysplasia. Exp Eye Res. 1980; 30(5): 575–591, https://doi.org/10.1016/0014-4835(80)90041-X. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Beltran WA, Hammond P, Acland GM, Aguirre GD. A frameshift mutation in RPGR exon ORF15 causes photoreceptor degeneration and inner retina remodeling in a model of X-linked retinitis pigmentosa. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2006; 47(4): 1669–1681, https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.05-0845. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Downs LM, Scott EM, Cideciyan AV, et al. Overlap of abnormal photoreceptor development and progressive degeneration in Leber congenital amaurosis caused by NPHP5 mutation. Human Mol Genet. 2016; 25(19): 4211–4226, https://doi.org/10.1093/hmg/ddw254. [CrossRef]
Grimes WN, Baudin J, Azevedo AW, Rieke F. Range, routing and kinetics of rod signaling in primate retina. ELife. 2018; 7: e38281, https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.38281. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Olman C, Ronen I, Ugurbil K, Kim D-S. Retinotopic mapping in cat visual cortex using high-field functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci Meth. 2003; 131(1): 161–170, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.08.009.
Spitschan M, Datta R, Stern AM, Brainard DH, Aguirre GK. Human visual cortex responses to rapid cone and melanopsin-directed flicker. J Neurosci. 2016; 36(5): 1471, https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1932-15.2016. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Mowat F, Petersen-Jones S, Williamson H, et al. Topographical characterization of cone photoreceptors and the area centralis of the canine retina. Mol Vis. 2008; 14: 2518–2527.. [PubMed]
Neitz J, Geist T, Jacobs G. Color vision in dog. Vis Neurosci. 1989; 3: 119–125, https://doi.org/10.1017/S0952523800004430. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Smithson H . S-cone psychophysics. Vis Neurosci. 2014; 31: 211–225, https://doi.org/10.1017/S0952523814000030. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Weisse I, Ganz H, Knappen F. Pupillometric studies in the beagle dog. Naunyn-Schmiedeberg Arch Pharmacol. 1975; 291(4): 385–394, https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00501796. [CrossRef]
Kim J, Heo J, Ji D, Kim M-S. Quantitative assessment of pupillary light reflex in normal and anesthetized dogs: a preliminary study. J Vet Med Sci. 2015; 77(4): 475–478, https://doi.org/10.1292/jvms.14-0387. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Aksenov DP, Li L, Miller MJ, Iordanescu G, Wyrwicz AM. Effects of anesthesia on BOLD signal and neuronal activity in the somatosensory cortex. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2015; 35(11): 1819–1826, https://doi.org/10.1038/jcbfm.2015.130. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Sicard KM, Duong TQ. Effects of hypoxia, hyperoxia, and hypercapnia on baseline and stimulus-evoked BOLD, CBF, and CMRO2 in spontaneously breathing animals. NeuroImage. 2005; 25(3): 850–858, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.010. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Iwabe S, Ying G-S, Aguirre GD, Beltran WA. Assessment of visual function and retinal structure following acute light exposure in the light sensitive T4R rhodopsin mutant dog. Exp Eye Res. 2016; 146: 341–353, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.exer.2016.04.006. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Aleman TS, Jacobson SG, Chico JD, et al. Impairment of the transient pupillary light reflex in Rpe65 −/− mice and humans with Leber congenital amaurosis. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2004; 45(4): 1259–1271, https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.03-1230. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Jacobson SG, Cideciyan AV, Sumaroka A, et al. Defining outcomes for clinical trials of Leber congenital amaurosis caused by GUCY2D mutations. Am J Ophthalmol. 2017; 177: 44–57, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajo.2017.02.003. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1.
 
(a) Signals from the L/M (hereafter “L”) and S cones are combined and contrasted to create two postretinal channels that signal luminance and chrominance. (b) Spectral sensitivities of the canine photoreceptors. (c) Stimulus spectra used to target particular photoreceptor combinations. Each stimulus modulated between a stimulating (red) and suppressing (black) spectrum around a common background (dashed gray). The nominal contrast on the targeted photoreceptors is given in brackets. Light flux produces equal contrast on all photoreceptors. (d) A spatially uniform field of light was presented to one eye. The field modulated between a stimulating and a suppressing spectrum, following a sinusoidal temporal waveform. In measurements of pupil response the modulation frequency was 1/6 Hz, and the consensual pupil response to the stimulus was recorded from the unstimulated eye using an infrared camera. (e) In fMRI measurements, a higher frequency flicker was presented in 12-second blocks, interleaved with presentations of the static, photopic background spectrum.
Figure 1.
 
(a) Signals from the L/M (hereafter “L”) and S cones are combined and contrasted to create two postretinal channels that signal luminance and chrominance. (b) Spectral sensitivities of the canine photoreceptors. (c) Stimulus spectra used to target particular photoreceptor combinations. Each stimulus modulated between a stimulating (red) and suppressing (black) spectrum around a common background (dashed gray). The nominal contrast on the targeted photoreceptors is given in brackets. Light flux produces equal contrast on all photoreceptors. (d) A spatially uniform field of light was presented to one eye. The field modulated between a stimulating and a suppressing spectrum, following a sinusoidal temporal waveform. In measurements of pupil response the modulation frequency was 1/6 Hz, and the consensual pupil response to the stimulus was recorded from the unstimulated eye using an infrared camera. (e) In fMRI measurements, a higher frequency flicker was presented in 12-second blocks, interleaved with presentations of the static, photopic background spectrum.
Figure 2.
 
Mean (across animals) pupil response to three different stimulus conditions: light flux (which drives all photoreceptor classes), L+S (cone luminance, silencing the rods and melanopsin), and Rod+Mel (silencing the cones). Measurements were made for three groups of animals (WT, RCD1, and XLPRA2). The measured pupil response is in gray, and the fit of a sinusoidal response model is in red, with the thin lines indicating the standard error of the mean across all acquisitions from all animals in a group.
Figure 2.
 
Mean (across animals) pupil response to three different stimulus conditions: light flux (which drives all photoreceptor classes), L+S (cone luminance, silencing the rods and melanopsin), and Rod+Mel (silencing the cones). Measurements were made for three groups of animals (WT, RCD1, and XLPRA2). The measured pupil response is in gray, and the fit of a sinusoidal response model is in red, with the thin lines indicating the standard error of the mean across all acquisitions from all animals in a group.
Figure 3.
 
Polar representation of pupil responses to the three stimulus conditions for each studied animal. Distance from the origin indicates the amplitude of pupil change, and angular position indicates the phase delay. The standard error of the mean of the response measured for each animal (across acquisitions) is indicated by the error bars.
Figure 3.
 
Polar representation of pupil responses to the three stimulus conditions for each studied animal. Distance from the origin indicates the amplitude of pupil change, and angular position indicates the phase delay. The standard error of the mean of the response measured for each animal (across acquisitions) is indicated by the error bars.
Figure 4.
 
Flattened representation of the canine visual cortex. The marginal, supra-splenial, and splenial sulci are indicated. The dots show the expected position of the upper and lower vertical meridians (UVM, LVM) and the dashes show the horizontal meridian (HM). The foveal confluence is indicated with a star.
Figure 4.
 
Flattened representation of the canine visual cortex. The marginal, supra-splenial, and splenial sulci are indicated. The dots show the expected position of the upper and lower vertical meridians (UVM, LVM) and the dashes show the horizontal meridian (HM). The foveal confluence is indicated with a star.
Figure 5.
 
fMRI responses to cone-directed stimulation. The average, across animal (and hemisphere) response (percent signal change), is shown for each of three stimuli and each of four studied populations. Cortical responses are shown on a flat map, and subcortical response on an axial slice that passes through the LGN. Data from the two hemispheres were combined, resulting in symmetric maps. The location of the horizontal (dashes) and vertical (dots) meridians are indicated. Different map thresholds were used for the cortical surface and subcortical maps (see color bar). Responses in the cortex and thalamus are of opposite sign, perhaps owing to the effects of anesthesia.
Figure 5.
 
fMRI responses to cone-directed stimulation. The average, across animal (and hemisphere) response (percent signal change), is shown for each of three stimuli and each of four studied populations. Cortical responses are shown on a flat map, and subcortical response on an axial slice that passes through the LGN. Data from the two hemispheres were combined, resulting in symmetric maps. The location of the horizontal (dashes) and vertical (dots) meridians are indicated. Different map thresholds were used for the cortical surface and subcortical maps (see color bar). Responses in the cortex and thalamus are of opposite sign, perhaps owing to the effects of anesthesia.
Figure 6.
 
fMRI responses (% signal change) within the visual cortex (left) and LGN (right) for each of the three stimulus types (rows), organized by disease model. Error bars are the standard error of the mean across fMRI acquisitions obtained for an individual animal. Note the different y-axis direction and range for the cortex and LGN plots.
Figure 6.
 
fMRI responses (% signal change) within the visual cortex (left) and LGN (right) for each of the three stimulus types (rows), organized by disease model. Error bars are the standard error of the mean across fMRI acquisitions obtained for an individual animal. Note the different y-axis direction and range for the cortex and LGN plots.
Figure 7.
 
fMRI responses (percent signal change) to light flux stimulation within the visual cortex of a CRD2 animal following gene therapy delivered to the left eye.
Figure 7.
 
fMRI responses (percent signal change) to light flux stimulation within the visual cortex of a CRD2 animal following gene therapy delivered to the left eye.
Table.
 
Summary of the Studied Animals
Table.
 
Summary of the Studied Animals
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×