The mean age for our cohort was 10.3 ± 2.4 years (range, 7–15 years). The diagnostic categories for the children included: primary congenital glaucoma, steroid-induced glaucoma, and juvenile open-angle glaucoma. Since the correlation between the MDs of right and left eyes from the HFA was low 0.31 (range, −0.07 to 0.61) consistent with differential monocular eye disease, we used each eye (
n = 32) as an independent observation for analysis. All children were retested (aged 11.0 ± 2.4 years) at a subsequent visit on average some 11.0 ± 6.4 months (range, 2–21 months) after their first visit as needed for their clinical management (
Fig. 2). The data from both tests contributed to the test–retest analysis except as indicated in the following.
Three children (five eyes [15.6%] of all eyes; ages 8, 9, and 13 years) could not complete the HFA SITA standard test at the first visit, but could do so in their fellow eye after having performed the MRF, which possibly provided them some familiarity and learning to perimetry testing. Over both the test and retest 11% (7 eyes) of eyes could not be tested on the HFA and 3.1% (2 eyes) could not be tested on the MRF. Possibly the more concerning prospect was that three eyes (9.4%) of three children, aged 13, 13, and 15 years, could not be tested at the retest visit despite a successful first test, indicating that a successful outcome does not guarantee future perimetric success in a child.
We determined the glaucoma severity score for the 32 eyes of the children based on the HFA MD index (normal, −2 dB < MD; mild −2.1 > MD >−6 dB; moderate −6.1 > MD > 12 dB; advanced −12.1 > MD > −20 dB; severe −20 dB > MD).
9 Here we used the HFA MD from the first successful test visit, which in most cases (27/32) was the first visit.
Figure 3 shows the distribution of the severity of visual field loss in our test cohort.
Including both test and retest, we achieved 57 results (89%) with HFA testing and 62 results (97%) with MRF.
Figure 4 shows the test times for 32 eyes on the MRF at the initial visit and for the 30 eyes that were able to be retested. It also shows the test times for the same children on HFA SITA standard (
n = 48) or SITA fast (
n = 9) over both the test and retest visits. Average MRF test times across both test sessions (5.6 ± 1.2 minutes) were similar to SITA Fast (5.6 ± 1.4 minutes) and both were significantly faster (approximately 3 minutes per eye) than SITA standard (8.3 ± 1.2 minutes;
P < 0.001).
We analyzed the concordance between the MD obtained with the first MRF test and the first HFA test for those 28 eyes for which we had data (
Fig. 5). These show excellent correlation (intraclass correlation coefficient = 0.91; 95% confidence interval, 0.82–0.96) with one outlier identified by an outlier test (
P < 0.05, GraphPad Prism:
https://www.graphpad.com/quickcalcs/Grubbs1.cfm) and indicated by the red-colored symbol in
Figure 5. Without this outlier, most of the data (86%) was within ±6 dB (dashed red diagonals) of the unity line (gray diagonal in
Fig. 5) and returned a Deming regression of
Y = 0.70*
X − 0.08 (thick dashed gray line). The gray symbols in the top right of
Figure 5 identify seven eyes that returned HFA MD values of greater than −2 dB and were considered as having normal visual fields. Of note, our data do not approximate the unity line in
Figure 5, but has a slope of 0.7. We believe that this shallower slope results from the larger test spot used in peripheral locations of the MRF as shown in
Figure 1. The four blue symbols identify children who lie more than 6 dB from the unity line: these all are found at high MD values consistent with the slope of 0.7. The raw data for the child shown as a blue square in
Figure 5 is presented in
Figure 6. Here it is evident that the MRF has higher thresholds (by approximately 5–7 dB) in the periphery owing to its larger spots particularly in the nasal region, even though the patterns of defect shown by the gray scales look similar.
Figure 7 shows the Bland–Altman analysis for the data of
Figure 5. The gray zone identifies the 95% limits of agreement for the seven eyes with normal MD and the bias is shown on the right. We undertook this analysis in eyes having normal MD because variability is known to increase as threshold decreases and we wanted to find out how children with normal visual fields would perform. The bias for these eyes was −0.6 dB with a 95% coefficient of repeatability (COR) of 2.2 dB (shaded region). These findings suggest that the MRF will return reliable estimates of HFA MD in children with normal thresholds. The small 95% COR (2.2 dB) is a useful measure of precision for clinicians as it defines the limit which will contain 95% of repeat test results, such as when testing a child over several visits.
Figure 7 also shows the 95% limits of agreement for the total group of eyes, excluding the outlier (dash horizontals). This has a bias of −2.7 dB confirming that the HFA returns a larger MD (more negative) and that there is a large range for 95% limits of agreement (4.3 to −9.7 dB) with 4 of 28 eyes (14%) of children producing outcomes that deviate beyond 6 dB of the HFA MD. Such deviation is most evident in children having advanced glaucoma (MD <−12 dB).
This lack of association may reflect high variability in MRF outcomes in advanced disease.
Figure 8 considers this prospect by showing the Bland–Altman analysis for test–retest made on the same device.
Figure 8A shows HFA test–retest data where little bias (1.8 dB) is evident and a 95% COR of 10.0 dB, these calculations exclude the outlier. In contrast,
Figure 8B shows that the MRF has a bias of 1.1 dB between test and retest and a 95% COR of 10.5 dB. Thus, the MRF data were repeatable, as were the data from the HFA in this cohort of children.
The pattern deviation from the first test of the MRF was found to have a nonlinear relationship with HFA PSD (
Fig. 9). What was evident was that the MRF has a larger pattern index than does the HFA particularly in some children having low PSD values for HFA (shaded region, <5 dB). In fact, 10 eyes (28%) had an abnormal pattern deviation on MRF in the presence of a normal or borderline PSD in the HFA (shaded region in
Fig. 9). Possibly early losses of visual threshold in children have a pattern in their loss and clinicians should look for the patterns and consider this index to identify them.
We checked the reliability of children in performing visual field tests on the two devices. Over the test and retest of 16 children (16 × 2 eyes × 2 tests), we achieved 62 viable tests for the MRF and 57 for the HFA. The HFA device flagged 19 of 57 outcomes (33%) as unreliable: 8 (14%) were flagged for high false-positive (FP) rates (>15%) and all 19 (33%) had excessive (>25%) fixation loss. The MRF flagged 23 of the 62 tests (37%) as unreliable: 13 (21%) with high FP rates and 16 (26%) with poor fixation using a criterion of 33% as unreliable (
Fig. 10). We found that test reliability is a function of age with unreliable outcomes more likely in children under the age of 12.5 years (
Fig. 8, vertical line). We also find that FPs have a significant downward slope (−2.9;
P = 0.01) for the linear regression against age, compared with fixation loss (FL), where the age-related slope was not significantly (−1.60;
P = 0.17) removed from zero.
The outcome of our survey after the second test session found that all children preferred the test experience on the MRF, reporting it easier and more agreeable than for the HFA. One 9-year-old child was uncooperative and refused to do the HFA test on their first exposure, resulting in a typical cauliflower defect (
Supplementary Fig. S1). This child successfully completed MRF testing at that same visit and likewise successfully completed HFA testing on their fellow eye after the MRF exposure.