The growth of the eye in early childhood is regulated by emmetropization, a process by which eyes outgrow their neonatal refractive errors—whether hyperopic or myopic—to attain an appropriate length given their optical (refractive) power.
1,2 In recent years, however, this process has been failing in more and more children. As a result, the prevalence of myopia has increased substantially.
3,4 Myopic eyes are too long given their optical power, which results in blurred distance vision unless corrected with spectacles or contact lenses. The increase in the prevalence of myopia has been too rapid to be explained by a change in genetics alone. Rather it must be due, at least in part, to changes in the visual environment and children's interaction with that environment.
5
Decades of research involving animal models have shown convincingly that the visual environment does indeed play a key role in emmetropization.
2 The research has shown that chickens, guinea pigs, marmosets, tree shrews, and more detect signals from retinal images that guide the growth of the developing eye.
6 For example, placing a negative lens in front of a young animal's eye (which produces images that are generally behind the retina) causes the eye to grow long and become myopic, whereas placing a positive lens before the eye (producing images in front of the retina) causes the eye to grow less and become hyperopic.
6,7 In addition, the intensity
8–11 and spectral composition
12 of the lighting environment in which animals are raised have been shown to affect eye growth and the likelihood of developing myopia.
Our understanding of the critical properties of the visual environment as they relate to the development of myopia in humans is poor. A significant limitation to gaining better understanding is the fact that one cannot ethically manipulate the visual environment of children as one can in animal studies. Furthermore, the conditions under which animals are reared in the laboratory do not mimic the natural environment in which children are raised. As a consequence, researchers have had to make indirect inferences from natural experiments in which one set of children happens to have been exposed to a different environment than another (eg, denser housing
13). This approach is, unfortunately, subject to confounds because children in the two groups may differ in other ways that may be known and cannot be factored out or are simply unknown. Another problem concerns how to quantify a child's visual environment. This has often been done with subjective reports (ie, parental questionnaires). Such data are potentially flawed in that parents may not know exactly how their children spend their time or may be biased in reporting it.
14,15 Nonetheless, these studies have yielded some insights. For example, children who spend more time outdoors are less likely to develop myopia, as reported in both cohort studies
16 and randomized control trials.
17,18 The outdoor environment differs from the indoor environment in the intensity and spectral composition of illumination and also in the distances of objects in the visual field. This leads to the hypothesis that the statistics of illumination and/or distance are a predictor of who will develop myopia. But without measuring such statistics directly in young children, one cannot assess their potential importance. Wearable technologies have been used to make such measurements in children, but, as we describe in the next section, they have not to date enabled the collection of the statistics we need to better understand risk factors. Thus, there is a compelling need to measure the visual environments of children comprehensively before they develop myopia. Such measurements should be made with a mobile device that does not prevent children from engaging in their everyday activities. The measurements should be made across a wide range of distances, including close distances because children spend significant time fixating near objects. Such distance measurements should be made across a reasonably wide field of view because of evidence that non-foveal retinal images (ie, in the near periphery) play a role in eye growth.
19 Measurements should also be made of the intensity and spectral distribution of illumination approaching the eyes, and those measurements should be valid both indoors and outdoors.